COGNITIVE
IMPAIRMENT IN THOUGHT REFORM
ENVIRONMENTS
The Faculty of the College of Education
Ohio University
In Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree
Doctor of Philosophy
by
Ronald Burks
March 4, 2002
This dissertation has been
approved for the
Department of Counseling and
Higher Education
and the College of Education
by
____________________________________________
Associate Professor,
Department of Counseling
and Higher Education�
____________________________________________
����������� Dean of the College of Education
����������� This
work and all my publications to this point, are dedicated to those to whom the
words of a pastor, guru, mentor or spiritual master have become wounds to the
soul.
����������� This
work is also dedicated to the memory of Louis Jolyon West, a true pioneer in
thought reform research and an example that those of us who work for human
rights must first be humans.� Our care
for those whose lives have been hijacked by another�s selfishness must reflect
our humanity.� So must our
research.� Researchers in human rights
work must be accountable for their bias and subject their work to rigorous
examination.� The cause of freedom of
thought can only be furthered with facts.�
Facts are more valuable than agendas.�
I hope this work furthers his ideals as well as his hope that no group
of people will ever be judged for their beliefs or practices, only held
accountable for the effects of those beliefs and practices.������
����������� Acknowledgements
����������� This
work would not have happened or this Ph. D. degree pursued, had it not been for
my good fortune to be able to work at Wellspring Retreat and Resource
Center.� Its director, Dr. Paul R.
Martin, is dedicated to rendering quality care and developing that care through
continuous research.
����������� Through
this association, I have been privileged to meet several research professionals
who have dedicated their careers to the pursuit of human rights through freedom
of thought.� Dr. Margaret Thaler Singer
who has labored in this stony field for over 50 years was gracious enough to
read the proposal when it was in process.�
Dr. Michael Langone, director of American Family Foundation, a research
and information resource organization who keeps encouraging researchers on four
continents to keep up the good work.
����������� Dr.
Peter Malinowski, whom I met when he was a first-year doctoral student in
Psychology at Ohio University and who has learned a thing or two about research
and statistics in the intervening years, read and provided valuable input.� Dr. Malinowski, Dr. Steve Lynn and others
developed with us at Wellspring what has become known on both sides of the
Atlantic as the Ohio Battery, a collection of measures through which the data
this study was collected. Dr. Steven Clay, faculty at the Ohio University
College of Osteopathic Medicine was read the drafts before they saw the light
of day so my glaring errors would be less noticeable.� My spouse, Vicki made sure my little errors, missing commas, wrong
words, weak sentences would be less noticeable.��
����������� And
finally, thanks to the faculty of the Counseling department, who prepare
students to be persons first, counselors second, and mirror that by
courageously being who they are.� Dr.
Davis, my committee chair, and advisor demonstrates how counselor education is
done.� Dr. Jerry Olsheski, Dr. George
Johanson, and Dr. Steven Patterson, who has carried on the interest of the
Department of Psychology in this field by graciously agreeing to participate in
this endeavor, have all made this a very positive learning experience.�
Title Page. Error! Bookmark not defined.
Chapter II - Review of the Literature
Introduction to the Literature
Critical Review of Relevant
Literature
Clinical Impressions of Ex �
members
Thought Reform or �Brainwashing�
Empirical Studies of Current
Members
Concerns with Studies of Current
Members
Concerns With Studies With Former
Members
Strengths and Weaknesses of the
Literature
How the Literature Supports the
Need for This Study
Operational Definition of the
Variables
Pre- and Post Treatment GPA Scores
Chapter V - Discussion of Implications, Conclusions and Recommendations
The Hypotheses of the Study:
Discussion and Comment
Veracity of Ex-member Recollections
Relative Contribution of Measures
of Affect
Discussion of Post Hoc Findings
Implications for Counselors and
Public Policy
Suggestions for Future Research
Appendix B - Consent form Used by Participants in this Study
Appendix C - Demographic Questionnaire
Appendix D - The �Ohio Battery� From Which This Study Was Taken
Appendix E - Boxplot of Post Hoc Study
List of Tables
1. Zero-Order
Partial Correlation Coefficients
2. Coefficients of Measures of Affect and a Measure of Thought
Reform
3. Comparison
of Mean Scores of Participants in Abusive Relationships. 42
Table of Figures
3. Histogram
of scores on the NIS-GMI subscale
4. Histogram
of Group Psychological Abuse scores
����������� Cults have existed for at least as long as humans have formed associations with each other (Singer & Lalich 1994). Langone (1993) reported that due to media coverage, the public�s perception was that the number of cults seemed to increase during the 1960's. By the late 1970's some ex-members began to present themselves for mental health treatment and researchers began to relate some of ex-member symptoms to their involvement in the cult (Clark, 1979; Singer & Ofshe, 1990).
����������� Other researchers claim to find the source of ex-member maladjustment on the characteristics of those who join cults (Spero, 1984; Levine & Salter, 1976).� They assert that pre-existing psychological problems, such as anxiety, unhappiness, egotism, depression, loneliness, rejection, sadness, meaninglessness and a lack of belonging as predictors of involvement in a cult.� Others suggest an unhappy childhood might lead one to join a cult (Ash 1985, Deutsch & Miller, 1983, Nicholi 1974).� These researchers suggest that cult members have nothing but their parents, their pasts, their present vulnerability, or even their pre-existing psychopathology to blame for being recruited into a cult and their distress is therefore, in most cases, not related to their cult experience.��
����������� Other investigators protest that this is nothing more than blaming the victim, a� practice they assert is not new to the mental health field.� Herman (1992b) alleged that for 60 years the mental health establishment covertly diagnosed survivors of childhood sexual abuse with having perverted unconscious drives and that it took political pressure to embarrass, cajole and otherwise force the needed research that changed the counseling paradigm from seductress/temptress vs. weak moral fiber/ male into the predator - prey model of today. She demonstrated that racial, gender, and national origin stereotyping and insensitivities were far too common in the counseling profession for more than three quarters of a century.�� Cult members and ex-members are the latest social group to be marginalized by stereotypes and ignorance.
����������� Research
on the effects of involvement in cults has been conducted into a range of issues:� family system predictors of later cult
involvement (Ash, 1985; Deutsch & Miller, 1983; Maron, 1988; Nicholi, 1974;
West & Singer, 1980), possible reasons people join (Latkin, 1990), and how
cults may affect people after they leave (Conway & Siegelman, 1982; Lewis
& Bromley, 1987; Goldberg & Goldberg, 1982; Singer & Ofshe, 1990;
West & Singer, 1980; Wright & Malony, 1989).� On a more sinister note, some researchers suggest (Galanti, 1993;
Hassan, 1988; Martin, 1993, West, 1990), as some in the popular press assume
(Stein, 2000), that brainwashing or other coercive persuasion occurs in cults.
This last question introduces the focus of this study.� Tragedies like Jonestown and Waco suggest
that some members of cults do things they might not otherwise have done, had it
not been for their involvement with the cult (Singer & Lalich 1994), even
participate in murder (State of Ohio vs. Daniel D. Kraft, Jr.).� Singer and Lalich suggest cults might have
the ability to get their members to behave in ways that violate members� the
basic values. Membership in some groups appears to inhibit clear thinking and
thoughtful behavior.� Counselors have
studied cognitive deficits and behavior disorders for nearly a century using a
variety of instruments. The Neuropsychological Impairment Scale (NIS) was
selected for this study as a measure of cognitive impairment.�
What sets cults apart from
other groups that seem to strongly effect their members? Langone et al. (1992)
developed a 28-item questionnaire to assess the environment of groups, called
the Group Psychological Abuse Scale (GPA).�
This enabled them and others to begin to explore the differences between
mainstream groups and those that are sometimes called cults.� They developed their instrument around the
proposition that some form of thought reform, commonly known as brainwashing, a
concept which will be defined later, pervades the environment of a cult.� The GPA is intended in this study as a
measure of� the level or intensity of
brainwashing that occurs in a given group.
����������� The
problem that is the focus of this study connects both the above ideas, to
explore the relationship, if any, between exposure to a thought reform system, as
measured by the GPA, to scores on a measure of cognitive impairment, the NIS.
����������� The
events surrounding the disaster at Jonestown and the many highly publicized
events in the intervening decades, may seem to make this a moot question.� Sudden migrations of whole groups and mass
suicides would seem to indicate that members of cults appear to behave in ways
that suggest a lack of critical thinking.�
But, as the review of the literature will show, prominent scholars of
the study of religion and sociology disagree (Maloney 1994; Barker 1995).� They blame the public�s negative perception
of cults, or New Religious Movements, pre-existing psychopathology in the ex
members themselves, or even the influence of organizations established to
assist members of cults and their families for any ill effects reported by
ex-members.� In fact, astonishingly
little organized empirical investigation into the cult phenomenon has actually
occurred (Aronoff, et al. 2000).
����������� This
study seeks to explore the relationship between a dynamic of organizations
commonly thought of as cults, thought reform and cognitive function of their
members.� HA: The intensity of�
thought reform environments in groups is correlated the level of� cognitive impairment in ex-members as measured
by a standard neuropsychological test.
����������� This
hypothesis is an alternative to the null HO: Intensity of thought
reform environment does not correlate with level of cognitive impairment.�
A human rights issue. The literature reviewed in chapter II will indicate that significant numbers of the population of this country are involved in various ways with various kinds of cults.� If the large numbers of individuals indicated below are being systematically deprived of skills vital to critical thinking, serious questions about violations of basic human rights in cults need to be asked.� Beyond that, the cost to society and perhaps even to national security might be enormous.� Cults and their effects need to be studied.
A social issue.
Cults are a significant social issue.� Martin (1993) estimated that between two and three million people who were, at that time between the ages of 18 and 25 had been involved in cults.� The International Cult Research Group conducted a survey for the American Family Foundation in 1993 that projected that two to five million people in the United States are involved in cults at any given time. Others estimate that as many as twenty million people could be involved.� The extreme range is accounted for by the difficulty in counting the number of groups themselves.� Estimates range from two to five thousand� groups that are alleged to be cultic (Martin 1993).� Using the most conservative estimates, cults, if they are were a disease, they would be the number four most common malady affecting the American population.�
����������� Assuming that cults have no actual negative impact on their members, the social stigma alone that the ex-member has to face can be emotionally emaciating.� According to the statistics above, between one and 7 percent of the population of the United States is facing a life of secrecy and shame having to hide elements of their past from prospective employers and potential mates. It would seem that a social phenomenon of this magnitude should be studied.
A legal issue.
����������� The
impact of this research and, hopefully more comprehensive studies that follow,
on the legal rights of victims of thought reform as well as groups alleged to
have caused harm may be incalculable.�
At present, those engaged in the recovery of damages or charged with
crimes related to cultic influence must have sufficient resources to hire
expert witnesses in a case-by-case basis.�
In effect, victims are required to prove they were impaired in order to
obtain either just recompense or mercy from the court.� This places judges in the legally awkward
position of requiring either victims or alleged perpetrators to prove their
innocence.� The law recognizes the
concept of reduced responsibility due to undue influence and a growing body of
cases exist that could contribute to legal precedent.� The paucity of general research into the dynamics of cults leaves
judges, attorneys and those they must judge and represent in a precarious
position.� Clearly, more knowledge in
this area would have the benefit of increasing the likelihood of fair treatment
by the legal system of both ex-members and the groups themselves.� Such important issues, at the core of this
country�s system of justice, will and should be settled by the collective
contribution of many studies.���
����������� Ayella
(1990) asserts that research into the cult phenomenon is difficult. Critics of
research into cult dynamics assert that thought reform or brainwashing is a
fictitious concept and they assert that it has never been subjected to scientific
scrutiny in controlled, laboratory conditions (Barker 1995).� No experimental research has been conducted
since the early 1960's in this field because modern research ethics do not
permit it.� Ironically, thought reform
researchers themselves realized the high potential for lasting harm to research
participants (Singer & Lalich, 1994) and influenced modern principles of
research ethics.� Robert Lifton (1997),
who initially identified the proponents of modern thought reform theory,
testified for the plaintiffs in litigation brought by research participants
against Central Intelligence Agency-supported brainwashing experiments by a
Canadian psychiatrist.�� It is
reasonable to conclude that if brainwashing exists, it must be harmful. It
would also be hard to define.
����������� The
practical impossibility of gathering time zero data is another serious
limitation of research on cults.� It is
not possible to measure the cognitive function of persons before they join.� As the literature review will show, anyone
from any background could potentially join a cult at some time in life.� Cults or any other groups that rely on
recruiting are understandably reluctant to assist with research into their
newest members or interested potential members. The risk of �turning them off�
is too great. Reports of longer term members are likely to be colored by bias
in favor of the group.� Research must
depend on recollections of former members, who might be biased against the
group due to the fact that they chose to leave.
����������� Another
problem with research using former members is the impossibility of determining
the total population of ex-members and finding a satisfactorily random sample
of them.� Negative stereotypes about the
kind of person that joins a cult would logically make it unlikely that
ex-members would be interested in identifying themselves unless they felt they
had something to gain in doing so.�
����������� Those
ex-members who do speak out appear to fall into two sometimes overlapping
categories, those who feel they have been exploited in some way and wish to get
back at the cult either in the media or in court and those who are so
emotionally wounded by their experience they sought treatment for it.� Neither group could be said to be non-biased
reporters. Further, critics of cult researchers complain that those who treat
ex-members of cults influence how ex-members recall the conditions in the
group.� While any study could not
possibly address all of these concerns, this study attempts to address this
last source of bias, treatment effect in its design.
����������� This
study is based on the responses of individuals who have presented themselves
for treatment at Wellspring Retreat and Resource Center, a residential
treatment center that specializes in the treatment of ex-members of cults and
those from one-on-one relationships that have similar characteristics.� Those persons however, should be seen to be
self-selected from the total population of ex-members of cults, in several
ways.�
����������� First,
participants felt their problems related to the cult were sufficiently serious
to warrant traveling to Southeast Ohio from as far away as Europe and
Australia.� No participants in the study
were legally mandated into treatment.�
All participated in treatment and the study by their own expressed
choice.� The possibility of bias against
the cult by participants must be taken into account.�
����������� Second,
the difficulty of finding Wellspring at all could be seen as a significant
delimitation of this study. Most people find out about Wellspring through
various networks of ex-members.� In
addition, while Wellspring has fairly extensive coverage in the media, on the
other hand, that coverage is highly targeted.�
Wellspring has been featured only in programs that reach restricted
audiences, such as daytime talk shows, prime time documentary news shows and
public service programs on state-run media in several countries.� A fairly large percentage found Wellspring
on the Internet.� Presumably, large
demographic sectors of ex-members without ex-member contact, interest in talk
shows, world affairs or access to the Internet would never know of its
existence.� Clearly, the results of this
study could not be generalizable to all ex-members of cults, and certainly not
to all current members.�
����������� The
results of any study of ex-members who seek either treatment or a forum to
network with other ex-members would presumably exclude two important and
impossible to measure groups, those ex-members who feel they have benefited
from their cult experience and moved on for one reason or another and those
members who remain in cults because they feel it is in their best
interest.� In other words, not only is
the sample biased, it excludes those whose experience may have been positive.
����������� On
the other hand, this study intends to use data supplied by ex-members of cults
who are impaired enough to seek treatment.�
It would seem reasonable that those who might have been harmed by some
phenomena should be the first to be studied.
Cult.������
For the purpose of this study, the term cult is defined operationally as any group to which an individual in this study belonged, that had characteristics that participant rated in such a way as to achieve a score on the Group Psychological Abuse Scale greater than 84.� This cutoff score indicates that conditions in the group were likely to have been a thought reform system and that the ex-member had been exposed to some degree of brainwashing.�
����������� An
operational definition is necessary (Aronoff et al., 2000) since the word cult
is thought to be pejorative.� The word
cult is used loosely to refer to a subculture, that is, any group outside the
mainstream of a cult-ure.� The word cult
cannot be applied to a group without assuming a negative connotation. This study
does not propose to sample from groups only because they are out of the
mainstream.� No individuals were part of
this study simply because they thought their former group was� �bad� or harmful.� This study assumes that there are many harmful and dangerous
groups that do not fit the profile that is of interest in this study.� In addition, groups could exist that are
generally benign that would be included.
����������� The
confusion is magnified by the incredible diversity of beliefs of groups that
could be called cults even under the very restrictive definition proposed
above.� By far the most familiar are
those groups with some form of religious perspective.� These could be classified by that mainstream world religion that
has the predominant influence on their beliefs.
����������� For
instance, the most widely known category of cults in the west probably includes
those groups whose primary influence is from Christianity.� They are referred to a �Bible based� groups.� These constitute a broad spectrum of belief
that would include far-right fundamentalist groups to those groups that embrace
a range of concepts from Judaism, Animism, Hinduism, Buddhism, or Islam, while
keeping a majority of emphasis on the Bible.�
����������� A
second broad category contains groups referred to as �Eastern based.� Their
main body of belief is adopted from Indo-Asian religious traditions.� Considerable overlap may occur between these
categories as groups borrow freely from both East and West.�
����������� A
third category consists of political/ideological groups that are characterized
by allegiance to a given geopolitical, racial, or social perspective (Langone,
1993).� Fourth would be the
business/motivational groups that generally tend to use the work of popular
sales motivational speakers as more or less sacred writings.� Fifth are the psychology/counseling/
psychotherapy groups that form around a strong, charismatic mental health
professional who violates client boundaries and numerous other ethical
constraints, usually forming therapeutic communities based on their unique
perspectives (Hochman, 1990; Singer, Temerlin, and Langone, 1990).
����������� The developers of the GPA (Langone, Dole & Wiltrout, 1992) were not interested in assessing the comparative benefits or deficits in a given group�s beliefs.� The theoretical foundation of the instrument is the interplay between belief and practice that fits a relatively narrow perspective they referred to as thought reform or �brainwashing.�
Brainwashing.
Thought reform or
�brainwashing� refers to a specific set of procedures or conditions this study
alleges to be used by cults.� Initially
described by Robert Lifton (1997) in his study of Korean prisoners of war,
these conditions are listed in the next chapter.� Lifton found that individuals exposed to these methods were far
more likely to be more compliant with the demands of the interrogators that
those who were physically beaten or abused. Ten percent of those exposed to the
procedures, an astonishingly high number given the times and the prevailing
patriotism of pilots and others who actually made public �confessions� to
outrageous allegations.� Many actually
had temporary feelings that they had �done the right thing.�
����������� The
term thought reform comes from a speech given by Mao Tse-Tung during the years
of communist party struggle against Nationalist forces.� Speaking against dissidents within the
party, he said, �these persons are doing counterrevolutionary deeds because
they are thinking counterrevolutionary thoughts.� Therefore, we must reform their thoughts�� (Singer & Lalich, 1994). In Chinese, the
concept of thought reform was represented by the characters, hsi, to reform or cleanse and nao, thought or brain or mind.� Hsi
nao could also be literally translated, �wash brain.�� American journalist Edward Hunter made the
connection and invented the English word �brainwashing� to describe the
techniques used in the many Thought Reform camps scattered throughout China
(Hassan, 1988).�
����������� The
term brainwashing is used loosely in popular exchange to indicate some
interrogation process that presumably makes the brain a blank slate with all
prior memories removed.� This notion
makes for entertaining plot lines for Hollywood but adds little to
understanding of cults, or other contexts where these techniques are used.� This study uses the historical definition
described in the next chapter and will use the less esoteric term, thought
reform (Lifton 1997).�� The connection
of the GPA to a recognizable, nonreligious, nonpartisan construct made it a
logical choice to operationalize the definition of �cult.�
Cognitive Impairment.
����������� Cognitive
impairment in this study refers to a range of factors related to cognitive
function as measured by the Global Measure of Impairment scale of the
Neuropsychological Impairment Scale, NIS (O�Donnell, DeSoto, Desoto, &
Reynolds, 1994a), an instrument developed to monitor progress or deterioration
in cognitive function in individuals with a variety of organic conditions.� Use of this instrument with a population,
suspected, but not known to suffer from organic conditions has some precedent
(Avants, Margolin, McMahon, & Kosten, 1997; Losasso, 1999).� It has been shown to correlate well with
more difficult to administer measures (O�Donnell, et al., 1994a) and its self
report form allowed much more data to be gathered than with traditional
intelligence measures. It is described in detail in chapter III.
����������� This
study proposes to investigate the connection between the intensity of a thought
reform environment to which a participant was exposed and their corresponding cognitive
impairment.� While a causal link between
the two could not be established without more controlled experimental
conditions, in the event a strong association is shown, further research is
indicated.� A response from the
healthcare community should be no different from that brought about from any
other consumer concern, i.e. automotive safety or suspected toxic waste in the
environment.�
����������� Aronoff
et al. (2000) found that the literature on cults is filled with �varied
personal, political, and scientific agendas.�
Research into this field is complicated by the fact that not all who
study cults agree that there is a problem in need of study.� Writers who seem to support the intellectual
and behavioral diversity that cults represent (e.g., Alexander, 1983; Anthony
& Robbins, 1992; Coleman, 1984; Wright & Malony, 1989) refer to cults
as New Reli�gious Movements or NRM�s, a neutral term without the negative
connotations connected with the term cult (Aronoff, et al. 2000). They assert
that they have the same First Amendment rights to freedom of expression
guaranteed to any other group.� �(e.g.,
Alexander, 1983; Anthony & Robbins, 1992; Wright & Malony, 1989) and
point out the positive effects cults have on participants (Galanter, Rabkin,
Rabkin, & Deutsch, 1979).
����������� �The
critics of cults view the cult phenomenon as essentially a human rights
issue.� They consider groups that use
techniques they recognize as similar to thought reform (Lifton, 1997) are
violating the basic human rights of their members. They assert that thought
reform processes appear to be related to lasting psychopathology after
individuals leave the group.
����������� The
critics of cults (e.g., Glass, Kirsch, Parris, 1977; Martin, 1993; Singer &
Ofshe, 1990; West, 1993; West & Martin, 1994),� point out that ex-members they have interviewed often experience
a wide range of psychological symptoms severe enough to impair social and
occupational function, including depression, anxi�ety, dissociation, passivity,
guilt, and panic attacks (Singer & Ofshe, 1990).�
����������� The
electronic database Psyclnfo was used to locate pertinent books and
articles. The keyword search included the following terms: cultism, brainwash,
brainwash�ing, mind control.�� The names
of authors who frequently publish arti�cles in the field were also included in
the search.
Introduction to the Literature
����������� This
search uncovered a large number of articles from both perspectives presented above
that appear to be opinion pieces, quoting favorable sources and making
assertions that stimulate further discussion but not necessarily
knowledge.� More research-oriented
articles are often based solely on interviews that gather what is essentially anecdotal
data. With few exceptions, studies are also flawed by the use unique
instruments designed only for the study with questionable validity and
reliability (Aronoff et al., 2000).�
����������� Access
to subjects for study further complicates research on cults. �Studies reported below will suggest that
current members of cults experience satisfactory levels of mental health and
are well adjusted.� Aronoff et al.
(2000) caution that studies of current members might consist of subjects who
were hand selected by their leaders to give the best possible impression of the
group, or who might hide their difficulties in order to avoid negative
repercussions, either immediate, from their leaders or eternal, since reporting
problems might violate the cosmology of the group.�
����������� Studies
with ex-members may also be biased in several ways.� The experimenter may have been a member of a cult and might be
more sympathetic to ex-members who report negative experiences.� Ex-members who are undergoing treatment may
experience more negative symptoms than other ex-members.� Counseling itself has been shown to have
negative effects on some clients (Crown, 1983; Lambert & Bergin,
1994).�
����������� Lack
of relevant control or non-cult groups is an ongoing weakness of cult
research.� Lewis and Bromley (1987)
reported a range of percentages of ex-members who experienced clinically
significant symptoms.� The low end was
27%, apparently quite high.� It must be
remembered, however, that the base rate for clinically significant symptoms in
the general population is a considerable 17% (National Comorbidity Study:
Kessler et al. 1994.���
����������� Another
complicating factor in research on cults is that no reliable estimate of the
total number of people involved with cultic groups exists.� Conventional polling procedures are of
little use since members of cults do not see themselves as members of
cults.� Estimates can only be derived
from local surveys and polls.� For
instance, the best estimate is that between two and five million Americans have
been involved in cults. This figure was derived from surveys of members of new
religious and parareligious movements in San Francisco and Montreal (Bird &
Reimer, 1982), surveys of high school students in the San Francisco Bay area
(Zimbardo & Hart�ley, 1984), a recent survey (1993) conducted by the ICR
Survey Research Group for the American Family Foundation, and a survey of 1,396
primary care physicians members of Pennsylvania Medical Society, 2.2% of whom
reported having had a family member involved with a cultic group (Lottick,
1993). Martin (1993) used two other surveys to estimate that there may be
anywhere from 2,000 to 5,000 cultic groups in America, with as many as two to
20 million members. Martin�s (1993) is far higher than previous estimates, but
is based on two different university surveys.�
Even using the most conservative estimates, cults are worthy of study.
����������� This
review of the literature will examine studies of current members of cults,
studies of former members and then turn to the literature that addresses the
dynamics of how cults work.�
Critical Review of Relevant Literature
Clinical Impressions of Ex � members
����������� Singer
and Offshe (1990) identified areas they felt were common characteristics of ex
members of cults.� Most ex‑cult
members Singer interviewed struggled with depression, loneliness,
indecisiveness, slipping into altered states, trigger experiences, floating,
subtle cognitive inefficiencies, fear of the cult, and the feeling of being in
a �fish bowl� constantly watched by family and friends.�� Having to repeat explanations of why they
joined and why they stayed gradually became agonizing for them.� Ex-members often feel guilt over having
brought difficulty into the family or over having recruited others into the
group. To make matters worse, the ex-member now realizes he or she was not part
of an elite, an historic or cosmic company on a mission to save the world.
����������� Their
work was exploratory, not empirical.��
Their impressions, drawn from thousands of interviews of ex-members,
contain analyzes that distill a very wide spectrum of symptomology into
concepts that are straightforward to handle, and presumably for this study, to
quantify. Their work underscores that cults represent a problem to society and
support the need for further research. This study is an attempt to quantify
their observation of reduced cognitive efficiency in ex-members.� Direct application of their work to what is
known about cults is limited by the lack of standard interview instruments and
standard, normed measures.� This study
will use an instrument with known psychometric properties to investigate their
observations empirically.
Thought Reform or �Brainwashing�
����������� Thought
reform or �brainwashing� was considered by several researchers (Hassan, 1988;
Singer & Ofshe, 1990; Galanti, 1993; Martin, 1993; West & Martin, 1994)
to be a key factor in understanding the profound changes that members of cults
experience.� Thought reform was first
described by Robert J. Lifton, Edgar Schein, Margaret Singer, and Louis Jolyon
West who in�terviewed dissidents from the Chinese Communist Workers Party who
had emigrated to Hong Kong and former prisoners of war (Galanti, 1993; Hassan,
1988; Martin, 1993).
����������� Robert
Lifton (1997) described eight interpersonal dynamics from his research with
former prisoners after the Korean Conflict and survivors of �thought reform�
camps in China.� In the introduction to
the latest edition of his book Thought
reform and the psychology of totalism, expressed his agreement, from his
own study of cults that his dynamics could be applied to them.� Individuals Lifton interviewed who had been
subjected to these eight processes could be influenced to adopt new ways of
looking at self or the world enabling them to make rapid changes in convictions,
values, attitudes and behavior.�
��������������� Milieu
Control
����������� The
foundation of thought reform is the establishment of a power differential
between the new inductee and the person or group. This position of greater
power is accomplished initially through the imposition� Milieu Control or control of the
surroundings.� The individual is
gradually denied contact with conflicting ideas or alternative explanations to
the phenomena observed in the group or those supposed explained by the groups
principles.� Milieu Control can be
accomplished through physical separation of the inductee from friends, family
and alternative source of information.�
In concert with the other seven processes, physical control of
surroundings is often unnecessary after the processes take effect.� A kind of psychological separation occurs, a
loss interest or outright fear of considering competing ideas.
��������������� Mystical
Manipulation
����������� Mystical
Manipulation is the ability of the group to induce states of emotional arousal
and, eventually self-deprecation and fear.�
Gradually, the group gains control of almost all the events or concepts
that regulate mood.� The individual
feels elation when exposed to an exercise supposedly unique to the group and
the resulting elevated mood is framed as a logical response to rightly applying
the principles of the group.� In like
manner, any thoughts expressed by the inductee in opposition to the teachings
of the group are deflected into personal challenges for �growth� and deeper
understanding.� Opposing ideas are
framed as lacking wisdom. Continued exposure to Mystical Manipulation results
in what Lifton called the �psychology of the pawn.�� The inductee feels trapped in a world ruled by forces that only
the group understands and that only the group has the means of controlling, the
Sacred Science.
��������������� Sacred
Science
����������� The
inductee is presented with a package without his or her knowledge or informed
consent that represents that portion of the beliefs of the group or Sacred
Science, that the inductee could be expected to accept given the positive
experiences felt during Mystical Manipulation.�
The result is that the individual is given the group�s interpretations
of reality that seem to confirm what the inductee has already experienced
emotionally.� This in the psychological
equivalent of the financial fraud known as the Ponzi Scheme,� where initial investors are paid the
promised generous returns out of the investments of later investors. Like the
early investor, the inductee has an emotional experience that confirms at least
some aspects of the Sacred Science are true. The Sacred Science always contains
the usually unexpressed ethic of the ends justify the means.� The ends of promoting the group and its
Sacred Science are so vital to life on Earth, that any means necessary are
considered ethical for either furthering the interests of the group or stopping
the efforts of those the group identifies as its enemies.�
��������������� Loading
the Language
����������� The
second part of the package is the language of the Sacred Science itself.� It is presented by using loaded terms and
phrases more laden with emotion than content using �bumper-sticker� slogans
that are designed to sound like jargon.�
Actually, they are �thought terminating clich�s.�� Lifton (1997) quotes Lionel Trilling who
calls this Loading the Language �the language of non-thought.� By controlling
the language the inductee is allowed to use to talk about the Sacred Science,
the group gradually manages to also control how the inductee thinks about the
Sacred Science.�
��������������� Cult
of Confession
����������� The Sacred Science is presented as a completed whole.� It appears that all others in the group accept it self-evident without the need for involved explanations, since all present have had experiences that seem to confirm its veracity.� Any doubts still plaguing the inductee are reframed as shortcomings of character that must be �confessed� as personal flaws, further removing the Sacred Science itself from the light of reasonable scrutiny.� The world is viewed by the group in simplistic, black and white categories and any failure to measure up perfectly with the demands of the Sacred Science must be confessed. Any material presented in this Cult of Confession can and will be used against the inductee as a Demand for Purity.
��������������� Demand
for Purity
����������� The
Demand for Purity is based on the notion that absolute perfection is possible
and any deviation from perfection is intolerable.� The inner world of the inductee is slowly reduced to endless
rounds of guilt and shame.� The cycle of
the Cult of Confession and Demand for Purity gradually wears down any sense of
self-acceptance that might have been present prior to the onset of the
process.� Eventually, inductees lose the
ability to believe they can make sense of the events they experience, choosing
the doctrines of the group as more trustworthy than their own experience in
defining reality.�
��������������� Doctrine
Over Person
����������� Doctrine
Over Person is the acquired ability of the inductee to reframe the events of
life only in the context of the teachings of the group, the Sacred Science.
Those experiences that support the Sacred Science are accepted, those that do
not are rejected.� Eventually the
inductee rewrites their personal history to force it into compliance with the
Sacred Science. When the group is faced with circumstances that force it to go
against its own Sacred Science, it constructs an often elaborate explanation
defining the circumstance as an artifact of the collapsing world system that
it, of course, predicted.� The apparent
hypocrisy of the group is redefined as evidence that the Sacred Science is more
true than physical reality. When the inductee has reached the point in the
process where personal experience is denied and doctrine accepted as reality,
all but the boldest machinations of the group are explained away by self-blame
or further rejection of the world outside the group.
��������������� Dispensing
of Existence
����������� As
a sense of self is squeezed, the need to connect with the group increases.� At the same time, the group makes it clear
that it alone has the wisdom and authority to decide who is qualified to exist on
the superior plane the group defines and who is not.� This develops naturally from the belief that the group has the
inside track on ultimate knowledge and as such, represents the only hope of
humankind.� All others, by definition,
do not measure up and in failing to do so, threaten the forward progress of
humanity, or the purposes of God, etc.�
The group freely engages in this Dispensing of Existence, making it
clear that the group is the elite, having more right to exist than
non-members.� Usually practiced
non-literally, groups Dispense of Existence through denouncing their detractors
and especially their defectors, usually by Loading the Language.���
����������� These
eight interpersonal dynamics describe an environment carefully controlled to
produce a particular response from participants, not just obedience but total
commitment to the goals of the group.
����������� Thought
reform and its implications formed the foundation of definitions of the word
�cult� that remove doctrinal or belief schema that obscured empirical research.� Singer and Lalich (1994) distinguished cults
from other groups by three factors, the origin of the group and the role of its
leader, the relationship between the leader and the group�s members, and the
existence of a coordinated program of persuasion.�� Langone (1993) related thought reform to cults in his
definition:
..a group or movement that,
to a significant degree, (a) exhibits great or excessive devotion or dedication
to some person, idea, or thing, (b) uses a thought reform program to persuade,
control, and socialize members (i.e., to integrate them into the group�s unique
pattern of relationships, beliefs, values, and practices), (c) systematically
induces states of psychological dependency in members, (d)exploits members to
advance the leadership�s goals, and (e) causes psychological harm to members,
their families, and the community.
����������� Aronoff
& Lynn (1996) pointed out that these definitions describe cults in a
negative fashion, presumably introducing bias to any investigation based on them.
It may not be possible to use the definitions to determine if any particular
group was a cult.� A perfect definition
may be elusive and that there may even be a continuum along which groups may be
distributed ranging from completely non-cultic to extremely cultic.� Another consideration was the unique
experiences of any two individuals in the same group might be completely
different, and could also be distributed on a continuum.� Those who were deeply involved might be the
only source of information on the cultic nature of the group.�
����������� The
Group Psychological Abuse Scale was developed on the conceptual foundation of
the above definitions (Langone, Dole & Wiltrout, 1992).� In order to operationalize the definition of
�cult� into a measure of individual experience for this study, the GPA was
chosen as a measure of the intensity of the participant�s exposure to a cultic
environment.�
����������� Theoretical
constructs and clinical observations are crucial to social science research,
and researchers who performed empirical studies reveal their theoretical
orientations in their experimental design.
Empirical Studies of Current Members
����������� Robbins
and Anthony, (1972) in the role of participant-observers, interviewed members
of the Meher Baba cult and concluded that the group which recruited many of its
members from the �hippie� counterculture, promoted abstinence from substance
use, a return to the work ethic and a deeper acceptance and appreciation of
others.� No standardized measures were
used and other emotional factors were not studied.�
����������� The
ability of cults to curtail substance abuse was the focus two other
studies.�� Galanter and Buckley (1978)
administered a questionnaire on substance use and psychiatric symptoms they
developed to 119 members of the Divine Light Mission (DLM). Members were asked
to answer each item when they experienced the most symptoms or used the most
drugs for four 2-month periods of time,�
before joining the group, immediately before joining, immediately after
joining and in the two months before the assessment.� Drug use and psychiatric symptoms decreased across all designated
time periods.�� Authors did not report
validity or reliability data for the measures they used.
����������� Galanter,
Buckley, Deutsch, Rabkin, and Rabkin (1980) used the same time reporting system
in a study of 119 members of Divine Light Mission (DLM) and 37 members of the
Unification Church (Moonies). In this study, substance use only was
assessed.� Once again, use decreased in
members of both groups.� Members of the
Unification Church reported a greater decrease in their substance use.
����������� These
two studies failed to take into account that both groups strictly forbid
substance use in their members. One could assume that members who could not
curtail their use would have been expelled.�
If any of Galanter et al.�s subjects had actually used substances while
they were members of the group and valued their membership it would follow that
they would be reluctant to report that use.
����������� Galanter,
et al. (1979) found that current members of the Unification Church� reported less emotional well-being than
nonmembers on a 216 item questionnaire.�
Members did report a decline in distress the longer they were in the
group.� Ninety-one percent said that
their distress declined immediately after joining the group.� The authors interpreted the results to mean
the group provides long-term relief from distress.� The study did not indicate the makeup of the non-member sample.
����������� Again
in 1980, Galanter studied attendees of Unification Church workshops. Attendees
who became members experienced the same gradual reduction in emotional distress
but never reported the same level of emotional well-being as nonmembers.
����������� Levine
(1981) asserted that cults reduce anxiety, confusion, and stress in members and
they do not adversely affect their members any more than any other intense
movement.
����������� Ross
(1983) administered the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), the
General Health Questionnaire, and the Eyseneck Personality Questionnaire to 42
members of a Hare Krishna Temple.� All
scores were in the normal range and although problems began to appear in
individuals who had been members longer than one and one half years, those who
had been members longer than three years scored in the normal range.� He concluded that the popular view of the
group as mentally disordered was not supported.� Other studies of this group, (Weiss & Comrey, 1987; Levine
& Salter, 1976), reveal a different side.�
Weiss and Comrey
administered the Comrey Personality Scales (Comrey, 1970) to 226 current
members and 727 nonmembers. Members were found to assess as more compulsive and
distrustful than nonmembers, even though the distrustful scale was within the
normal range.� Member�s high
compulsivity scores may simply reflect the structured lifestyle of the
group.� Levine and Salter (1976)
included Krishna devotees in their study of 106 groups considered outside the
mainstream at the time.� When asked why
they stayed in the group, only 20% specified spiritual, transcendental, or
mystical reasons.� In-depth interviews
of eleven randomly selected members indicated that most displayed psychiatric
symptoms but most did not meet diagnostic criteria.� They did not indicate what the symptoms were or exactly how many
presented with symptoms.�
����������� Latkin
(1990) administered the Private Self Consciousness Scale to 232 members of the
Rajneesh commune and found that they engaged in more self-examination than the
instrument�s normative population. They tended to feel less discomfort around
others and had higher self esteem.�
These findings are in keeping with the tenets of the group which promote
self-examination, self-acceptance and personal growth.� Latkin (1990) concluded that their scores
indicated members have strong opinions and would be hard to persuade,
contradicting the assumption that those who join cults are gullible.� The findings only provide a �snapshot� of
some aspect of being hard to persuade.�
Latkin did not set out to measure susceptibility to persuasion
behaviorally, only drew this conclusion from combinations of scale scores.� A sample of interested visitors, presumably
members-to-be, might have made his conclusion more supportable.� The study did not include other measures
that might have provided a context for the hardening of opinions.� Membership in a group known to expect
allegiance without question might, in itself, account for elevations in
measures of strength of opinion.
����������� In
another study of 67 Rajneeshpuram (Sundberg, Latkin, Littman and Hagan, 1993),
devotee scores on the California Psychological Inventory suggested that
Rajneeshees are socially poised, independent, exhibit flexibility in thinking
and reject conforming to social standards.�
No data of direct interaction between the experimenters and subjects was
reported to confirm test results.�
����������� Ungerleider
and Wellisch (as cited in Aronoff, Lynn & Malinoski, 2000) compared 33
current and 17 ex-members of several unspecified groups using structured
interviews developed for the study by the authors, a mental status exam, the
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, short form, the MMPI, and the Interpersonal
Check List.�
����������� MMPI
Lie scale scores were considerably higher for current members than former
members.� Elevated Lie scale scores
indicate that the individual is answering questions in a socially desirable
light and minimizing any problems.� On
one part of the paper the authors reported that current members also had
elevations on scales 6 (Paranoia) and 8 (Schizophrenia), and former members had
elevations on scales 3 (Hysteria) and 4 (Psychopathic Deviate). In another
section, they reversed the scores, attributing high 6 and 8 scores to former
members and high 3 and 4 scores to current members.� The authors did not report actual scale scores eliminating any
possibility of review of the authors� interpretations or working around the
typographical error.
����������� Of
particular interest to this study, Spero (1984) administered the Verbal and
Performance sections of the Wechsler Memory Scale, (WMS), the Bender Gestalt,
the Rorschach and the Embedded Figures Test to 51 members of cults before and
after six months of counseling.� After
six months of counseling, WMS scores increased, the Bender scores indicated
more efficient processing of visual stimuli and less blocking.� Spero (1984) interpreted the results of the
Rorschach and the EFT to suggest that untreated members of cults might display
perceptual and cognitive inefficiencies.�
He asserted that these deficits were associated with �passivity,
identity confusion, other-orientedness,...� and an �unclear sense of separate
self...� .� A non-cult, control group
would have added considerable credibility to the study for measures this
complex.� The absence of direct measures
of affect undermines his conclusions.�
His study was drawn from a clinical population that would more likely to
be more distressed than regular members.�
Although he made use of standard instruments, he did not use consistent
methods for data collection or assessment making his work more anecdotal than
analytical.
Concerns with Studies of Current Members
����������� Most
of the studies reported so far suggest that membership in a cult can be
beneficial.� Members experienced relief
from drug addiction (Galanter & Buckley, 1978; Galanter, et al., 1980),
connection to others with similar interests (Galanter et al., 1980), and
reduction of distress (Galanter et al., 1979).�
Most studies (Galanter et al. 1979; 1980; Galanter & Buckley, 1978);
Levine & Salter, 1976; Spero, 1984) do not allow for comparison of cult
members with well-defined norming populations since they did not make use of
standardized measures with known validity and reliability (Aronoff, et al.,
2000).
����������� Ayella
(1990) lists methodological and other difficulties encountered in researching
cults. Researchers must face very real culture shock, difficult emotional
responses to the group, handle conversion attempts, and the stigma between
colleagues and others due to their investigating a group considered by many to
be deviant. Establishing and maintaining access to the group can be frustrating,
often hampering researchers� ability to obtain large enough �and random enough
samples of members.� Cults have much to
risk in allowing researchers to do their work without interference.� A good outcome could result in favorable
publicity, elevate the group�s social standing, and enhance its ability to
raise funds.� On the other hand, just
the fact it is being studied by researchers known to investigate cults could
negatively impact the group�s image.�
Leaders of cults are often overly sensitive to public opinion and are in
a position to influence access to the group, analysis of research, and
perceived credibility.
����������� Aronoff
et al. (2000) suggested the possibility that members may not report honestly on
instruments and that they might be influenced to actively under report their
difficulties because of pride in the group or distrust of the researcher who is
an outsider.� Researchers who do gain
access to groups may be able to do so because of a bias toward the group.� Levine and Salter (1976) observed that while
members reported greater feelings of emotional well-being after joining the
group on questionnaires, more in-depth interviews tended to uncover
considerable pathology. The doctrine of the group might indicate that there
will be cosmic or social repercussions for not being positive about themselves
or the group at all times (Swartling & Swartling, 1992).
����������� Studies
with former members of cults have relatively few of these problems, but the
next section will show that ex-member studies have their own shortcomings.
����������� Clinical
impressions of clinicians who have treated former cult members have influenced
empirical studies.� Aronoff et al.
(2000) reviewed the literature and found the most common symptom reported by
clinicians was dissociation. Dissociation for the ex-cultist is called
�floating� in the literature (Goldberg & Goldberg, 1982; West & Martin,
1994; West & Singer, 1980), the experience of profound ambivalence toward
the group after leaving, along with feelings of being in the milieu of the
group, even years after leaving. Cognitive deficiencies, particularly
difficulty making decisions and simplistic, black and white thinking were often
reported (Goldberg & Goldberg; Singer & Ofshe, 1990).� Depression�
(West & Singer; Wright & Malony, 1989) and anxiety (Singer &
Ofshe) were also commonly reported. Florid psychotic symptoms are less fre�quently
noted (Singer & Ofshe), though clinicians unfamiliar with cults sometimes
mistake reports of bizarre conditions in groups as delusional thinking.� These observations were made by mental
health professionals on individuals who sought their help for emotional issues.� Although these investigators were reporting
their impres�sions and not doing on empirical research per se, their insights
have influenced a new generation of researchers who began using
author-developed surveys and finally standardized psychometric instruments and
diagnostic interviews. Not many have been done and all have their flaws, but
all indicate that cult involvement comes with mental health risks.
����������� Surveys
developed by the authors were used to determine participants� impressions of
their emotional state (Conway & Siegelman, 1982; Lewis & Bromley,
1987).� Conway and Siegelman found that
seven symptoms (altered states, nightmares, inability to break mental rhythms
of chanting, amnesia, suicidal/self harm tendencies, hallucinations and violent
acting out) increased with time of involvement in the group.� Lewis and Bromley examined the same seven
symptoms as Conway and Siegelman and found no correlation between length of
membership and symptom intensity and frequency.
����������� Swartling
and Swartling (1992) asked 43 ex-members of Livits Ord or Word of Life, a group
in Sweden about symptoms they experienced after but not before joining the
group.� Ninety-three percent, reported
anxiety and guilt, (91%) reported difficulty handling their emotions, (88%)
felt empty, (96%) said they had nightmares or other disturbances of sleep,
(75%) had difficulty concentrating, (63%) reported suicidal thoughts, and (90%)
felt a loss of identity.� Before joining
the group, (16%) had consulted a psychiatrist but (63%) did so after leaving.� They conclude that former members report
that involvement in a cult increased their symptoms.
����������� Aronoff,
et al. (2000) reported that Wright and Malony (1989) and Galanter (1983) found
quite different tendencies in ex-members they studied.� Wright and Malony found ex-members� symptoms
fade quickly with time and Galanter found that ex-members do not report greater
psychopathology than nonmembers but that those who had been more or less
forcibly removed from their groups reported more negative feelings toward the
group.� His conclusion suggests the
intervention itself was responsible for increased reports of negative symptoms,
not the group involvement itself.
����������� These
studies have most of the same problems as those with current members,
nonstandard instruments, some lack control groups, and some appear to study
inadequate sample sizes.� They were
significant in that they served to cement researcher opinion into two
camps.� One side believed that groups
systematically harmed their members. The other side believed that groups are
generally benign, but counter cult interventionists create problems for
ex-members and bias them against their former group (Aronoff et al. 2000).
����������� In
1992, Martin, Langone, Dole and Wiltrout applied standard assessment
instruments to the study of ex-members.�
He and his colleagues administered the Millon Clinical Multiaxial
Inventory (MCMI; Millon, 1983), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Hopkins
Symptom Checklist (HSCL; Derogatis, et al., 1974), and the Staff Burnout Scale
(SBS-HP) to inpatients all of whom had sought out treatment at Wellspring Retreat
and Resource Center, a facility that combined a bed-and -breakfast residential
setting with a two-week program of individual counseling and workshops.� All participants had sought treatment on
their own or at the urging of significant others.� None were mandated into treatment by a court or other public
authority.�
����������� These
measures were administered di�rectly after the intake interview, a semi
structured set of two questionnaires developed by the author. The first
questionnaire gathered demographic information, including specific questions on
the par�ticipants� experience in the
group.� The second questionnaire was
based on general mental status interviews in common use at community mental
health agencies and hospitals, assessing current and past symptoms and physical
health. The Millon inventory was chosen in the belief that the distress felt by
former members was somewhat ego syntonic, resembling personality disorders, the
primary focus of the instrument.�
����������� A
score of 75 on any MCMI scale is regarded as clini�cally significant. One
hundred six of the 111 participants (95%) scored above 75 on at least one MCMI
scale. The scales with the highest means were: Anxiety (76), Dys�thymia (72),
and Dependent (Submissive) (72). Forty-two participants also completed the
HSCL.�� The mean score was 102, with
scores over 100 indicating a need for psychiatric care. Forty-six completed the
SBS-HP which specifies that a score above 70 indicates burnout and acute
stress.� The mean was 72.� Scores of 10 or more on the BDI are
considered to be outside the normal range and scores above 17 are indicative of
clinically significant depression (Langevin & Stancer, as cited in Martin,
et al. 1992).� Once again, the mean was
elevated, 14, not high enough to indicate a depressive disorder but outside the
normal range.
����������� Since
it was impossible to gather times one data, elevated scores, especially on the
MCMI could have been due to conditions in the participant�s experience that
existed prior to joining the group.� To
address this, the 111 participants were sent another MCMI six months after
treatment.� Fifty-nine and one-half
percent returned the materials.�
Participant scores decreased on the �Histrionic, Narcissistic, and Anti�social
Scales increased, and scores on the Schizoid, Avoidant, Dependent, Negativistic
Aggression, Schizotypal, Borderline, Anxiety, Somatoform, Hypomania, Dysthymia,
Alcohol Abuse, Psychotic Thinking, and Psychotic Depression scales.� The greatest decline from pre- to six months
post-treatment was on the Dependent (Submissive), Anxiety, and Dysthymia
subscales.� Before treatment, on these
three scales 58.2, 52.2, and 47.8% had scores greater than 75 on the MCMI,
respec�tively. After six months, only 28.4, 26.9, and 25.4% of participants had
scores greater than 75 on the Dependent (Submissive), Anxiety, and Dysthymia
subscales of the MCMI, respectively.
����������� These
results indicate those ex-members of cults who seek treatment at Wellspring
after leaving the cult exhibit clinically significant symptoms.� Six months after a two-week treatment
program they report significant improvement.�
This is significant particularly in that the MCMI is designed to assess
personality disorders, conditions that, by definition, change little over time.� Indeed, the Millon inventories have been
shown to be fairly stable over time (Hyer, Woods, Gruno & Boudewyns, 1989).
Martin et al. (1992) concluded that the six month scores indicate that the
admission scores did not indicate long-standing personality traits, but that
the environment of the cult tends to �produce and/or exacerbate
dependent-compulsive types.�
����������� Martin,
Aronoff, Zelikovsky, Malinoski, and Lynn (1996) conducted a fol�low-up to the
earlier Wellspring study. They assessed 110 different Wellspring clients.� They found that participants� highest means
were on the Dependent (Submissive) (71.54), Self-Defeating (73.65), and
Avoidant (74.97) subscales of the MCMI. Eighty-seven percent (87%) had
elevations (>75) on at least one scale.���
The mean HSCL score was 112.78, well over 100, the level where
psychiatric care is indicated.� The mean
score on the BDI was 19.77, where scores of 17 or more suggest a depressive dis�order.
These results were essentially identical to the initial study.
����������� �Aronoff
et al. (2000) note that the MCMI was constructed using a comparison group of
psychiatric patients, not a non-clinical group, and its use in this case could
be questioned.� They also note, however,
that the other two measures support the same conclusion.
����������� This review suggests that former members of cults report significant symptoms and current members do not. Aronoff et al. (2000) proposed several alternatives to explain this apparent difference.� First, it was only after leaving the cult that former members fully realize the effects of their involvement. (Conway & Siegelman, 1982; Conway, Siegelman, Carmichael, & Coggins, 1986; Galanter, 1983; Martin et al., 1992; Swart�ling & Swartling, 1992).�� Second, those who leave a system to which they had completely committed will experience distress when facing the loss of faith and when attempting to adjust to life without the group.� (Galanter, 1983).
��������������� A stressful family background was associated with involvement in a cult (Ash, 1985; Deutsch & Miller, 1983; Nicholi, 1974; West & Singer, 1980).� The cult serves as a replacement family and when members leave the feel stress upon return to the family of origin. Maron (1988), on the other hand, found that family background was not a likely factor in cult involvement.
��������������� Individuals still in a cult
may underreport symptoms� (Ayella, 1990;
Swartling & Swartling, 1992). Individuals who have left a cult my
exaggerate or fake symptoms. Self report measures used in studies do not have
subtle items and validity indices raising questions about the veracity of
ex-member reports (AyeIla, 1990).
����������� The
last explanation, that ex-members are faking, was found by Aronoff et al.
(2000) to be addressed in the literature three different ways.� Conway et al. (1986), Singer & Ofshe,
(1990) and Spero, (1984) stated that ex-members were often angry at the group
and its leaders.� They might be
motivated to fake or exaggerate their reports of symptoms in order to exact
revenge on the group.
����������� Bromley,
Shupe, & Ventimiglia, (1983), Coleman, (1984) Galanter, (1983) Lewis &
Bromley, (1987), and Solomon, (1983) alleged that contact with organizations
that support ex-members of cults and their families subtly pressure them to
report higher levels of pathology.
����������� The
generally negative view that the public has of cults (Anthony & Robbins,
1992; Barker, 1995; Lewis & Bromley, 1987; Robbins & An�thony, 1980;
Saliba, 1999; Shupe, Bromley, & Oliver, 1983) might persuade ex-members to
go along with society�s views, finding it in their best interest to make their
experiences seem as bad as the public perceives them to be.�
����������� Aronoff
and Lynn (1996) addressed these issues by assessing not only ex-members (n=45) of cults but also 58 college
students who presumably would be aware of the public�s negative view of
cults.� The college students were given
the same battery of instruments as the ex-members and told to simulate how they
thought cult members would answer the items.�
In addition the investigators included a control group of 56 college
students who were told only to answer the items honestly.� In general, simulators reported more extreme
scores than actual former members indicating it is unlikely that ex-members are
faking their answers.� In addition,
ex-members scored higher on the battery than non-simulators, the control
population. �
Concerns With Studies With Former Members
����������� Studies
of ex-members of cults indicate that they experience significant emotional
symptoms.� The general use of
nonstandard measures, lack of control groups and possible experimenter bias
suggests caution in interpreting these results.� Some of the experimenters were themselves former members of cults
and might have been more supportive of those who reported negative experiences
(Aronoff et al. 2000).�
����������� Another
concern with studies of ex-members is that most participants have presented
themselves for treatment some time after leaving the group.� Random or even representative sampling was
never achieved. Ex-members in treatment could hardly be considered a
representative sample and studies using them would tend to have exaggerated
results.
����������� The
literature on psychological trauma adds much to a perspective on cult dynamics.
Trauma� is usually thought of as an
assault on the emotional and cognitive resources of the human organism by an
overwhelming stressor. Examples include experiences and behaviors as a result
of situations in which the individual perceived his or her life was in eminent
danger such as war, automobile accidents, and natural disasters. (DSM-IV 1994).
Others (Herman 1992b, van der Kolk 1995, Lourie 1996), extend the study of
trauma to situations where the individual s conception of a predictable world
is shattered. Here is where the study of cults and psychological trauma meet in
the literature.
����������� Van
der Kolk (1995) maintains that human reaction to psychological trauma is the
result of specific biological changes that occur in the brain in response to
overwhelming stress. Using Vietnam-era veterans as subjects, he and others
discovered a link between the reenactment of trauma in recall of memory and the
release of endorphins, explaining a phenomenon of emotional numbing reported by
survivors from across a large spectrum of trauma. From there, a study of the
entire CNS response to overwhelming stress began. Studies were extended to
survivors of other forms of trauma.
����������� van
der Kolk observed that interpersonal trauma is more insidious and more damaging
that trauma from accidents and disasters. Trauma sustained in childhood is more
difficult to recover from as an adult than trauma suffered as an adult. Trauma
inflicted by a supposed caring relative or friend is more difficult to recover
from than fires or floods (Lourie 1996). When the source of the trauma is
evident and the blame is obviously on the perpetrator the victim is often
better able to reconstruct a safe conception of the world and integrate the
experience than the victim who is unsure of what happened. They usually
conclude they have only themselves to blame.
This discussion will focus
on interpersonal trauma, given that at least some emotional responses are
common to all forms of trauma. (van der Kolk 1995). Lourie (1996) describes the phenomenon of cumulative trauma where
no single event can be called overwhelming but taken together, the effects
experienced and described by the victim are pretty much the same. Her construct
agrees roughly with Herman�s (1992a) description of chronic trauma. If
membership in a cult is a form of trauma (Herman 1992a; Singer & Ofshe, 1990),
cognitive impairment might be a risk factor.
Strengths and Weaknesses of the Literature
����������� A
great deal of anecdotal and informed opinion material has been published over
the last 30 years.� Empirical studies provide
conflicting impressions of the effects of a cultic environment which should not
be surprising given the weaknesses explored in this review.
����������� Instruments
used were usually of questionable validity and reliability (Aronoff et al.,
2000).� Difficulties of access to
subjects for study makes time zero data almost impossible to gather.� Possible response bias of current members in
favor of their groups and of ex-members against their former group warns that
caution is warranted in interpreting results.�
Experimenter bias could be a factor, especially in ex-member studies are
carried out by investigators who are themselves, ex-members. The results of
studies of research participants who have presented themselves for treatment
may not be easily applicable to all ex-members of cults. In only one study was
a relevant control group utilized complicating interpretation of the results.
����������� While
results differed on whether members of groups are emotionally healthy, clearly
some ex-members exhibit significant pathology that cannot be accounted for by
pre-existing factors.� No studies were
found that conclude that membership in a cult has long term, positive effects
on those who leave.�
����������� This
study could not possibly overcome all these obstacles.� Data was gathered from ex-members who sought
treatment, limiting the applicability to the larger population of
ex-members.� Some of the weaknesses of
other studies were addressed in gathering data. Data will be utilized in a way
that addresses the problem of response bias in the recollections of ex-members.
����������� One
of the largest problems, that of definition (Aronoff et al., 2000) will be
solved by using already collected GPA scores as a measure of exposure to a
cultic system.�� This study addresses
their recommendation to operationalize the definition of a cult.� This study uses individual GPA scores as a
measure of cultic milieu/thought reform intensity, eliminating the need to
identify and label specific groups as cults.�
Several of the instruments used by Wellspring were standard measures
with known psychometric properties.�
Only those instruments will be used in this study. No control group will
be used since the norming populations of the instruments are known.
����������� This
study will use Wellspring client scores on the GPA and the NIS to determine
what effect, if any, exposure to a cultic environment has on cognitive
impairment. It will use test-retest scores on the GPA to attempt to control for
response bias.� Two measures of
emotional state, the BDI and the HSCL (dissociation screen only) will be used
to control for emotional contribution to elevations if any, in NIS scores.
How the Literature Supports the Need for This Study
����������� It
has already been observed that relatively few empirical studies have been conducted
on current or former members of cults. Clearly, far more are needed.� Studies reviewed for this study focused on
emotional symptoms, i.e., depression (West & Singer, 1980; Wright and
Malony, 1989), anxiety� (Singer &
Ofshe, 1990), and dissociation (Goldberg & Goldberg, 1982; West &
Martin, 1994; West & Singer, 1980), but few (Goldberg & Goldberg; Spero
1984) investigated cognitive issues.�
Goldberg and Goldberg did not present their work as empirical research,
only as clinical impressions from their extensive work with of ex-members.
Spero (1984) did not have a known measure of cultic environment intensity to
factor into his results with cognitive measures, nor did he have a self report
measure of cognitive impairment that could be given to large numbers of
participants.
����������� The
sample for this study originally included 132 ex-members of groups or
relationships identified by a group evaluation instrument to be former members
of groups that were high demand, and very likely to be using the thought reform
techniques defined in the first chapter.�
����������� The
age of these individuals was between 16 and 65 with a mean of 33.14 years (SD=
11.2). Forty-three were male and 88 were female. Eighty-six had been members of
groups that used the Bible in some way as a basis for belief or practice.
Twenty-six were ex-members of groups that embraced a predominately Eastern
worldview.� Thirteen had been in abusive
relationships or families that fit the admission criteria. Their scores were
analyzed for differences from those of ex-members of groups.� Four had been in either abusive adolescent
substance centers, or extremist political or business related groups. Two did
not answer the item.
����������� The
average number of years of formal education was 14.69, that is, over two years
of college.� One hundred twenty-two were
Caucasian and five were African American four were either Asian American or
other and one did not answer the item.�
The income level most often recorded was zero.� No consistent reason for this was available.� The mean annual income for those reporting
an income was $46,218 (n = 90). Socioeconomic status for the client�s families
of origin was not available, due to their age (33.14).� The religious backgrounds indicated by those
in the sample included 30 (Protestant�
fundamental), 35 (Protestant liberal), 27 (Roman Catholic),
6 (Jewish), 0 (Islamic), 2 (Eastern), 2 (reared in
group), 7 (other), and 3 (none).� These demographic data are subject to change as the study
progresses.
����������� All
were entering treatment at Wellspring Retreat and Resource Center for what they
felt were after effects of their experience in the group or relationship.� All entered treatment voluntarily and signed
a statement to that effect before testing.�
No clients were referred by courts or other agencies. The fee for
residential treatment at Wellspring is $5000 for the two week program. At the
time, Wellspring attempted to negotiate with some client�s insurance carriers
for treatment but very few who had insurance were covered for residential
treatment. The administrative staff estimated that about one half of all
clients at the time of data collection paid for their treatment with their own
or their families� resources and the other half were treated using funds raised
from donors.
����������� Clients
of Wellspring are screened over the telephone and thorough examination of
counseling records of referring professionals to include only those individuals
who have experienced some form of perceived psychological trauma perpetrated by
another person. Survivors of general combat situations, accidents, or natural
disasters, would not be considered appropriate for admission, even though they
had experienced trauma.
����������� Cases
involving a single abusive episode followed by some form of resolution are not
considered appropriate for admission.�
All clients who are admitted report having experienced repeated
emotional injury and being led to believe that it was their own fault or for
their own good.
����������� Clients
arrived at Wellspring each Sunday evening and received the test battery on
Monday morning. They completed the battery over the first few days after
arrival.� Batteries not completed by the
end of the third day of treatment were not included in this study. Participants
were included in the final count for the sample if all measures were
completed.� The sample was gradually
accumulated at the rate of one or two each week from November 1996 to September
of 2000.
All individuals were offered
three options. They could complete the battery and sign a consent form to
include their data in this study.�
Second, they could complete the tests for their own benefit and to
provide information to their clinicians but not for use in this study, or,
third,� they could elect not to take the
battery at all.
����������� All
were informed that only their clinician would have access to their profile for
treatment purposes and, at their request, would be glad to go over the profile
with them.� They were told the study
would not make use of their names and that only their answers and non-
identifiable demographic data would be used by researchers other than their
clinician.
����������� Policy
at Wellspring requires that client test profiles and completed batteries be
kept with the individual�s medical record in a locked cabinet.� All computer scoring files containing names
are kept under password control. Only aggregate results are reported in this
study and any reference to specific individuals will be by an arbitrarily
assigned test battery number.
The size of the sample was
determined by the number of cases available at the time of this writing. The
literature surveyed indicated that the effects of cultic involvement, while
taken as a whole, are sometimes debilitating for the ex-member, any single,
discrete effect is likely to be subtle (Martin, et al. 1992). Therefore, the
effect, if any, of exposure to a cultic system on cognitive impairment is
likely to be small.� Of course, a small
amount of cognitive impairment could present serious problems in higher-order
decision making. In order to minimize the likelihood of making a Type II error,
that is to maximize the possibility of finding a difference if there is one,
considerable statistical power must be available in the analysis (Stevens
1996).�
����������� Due
to the controversial nature of this research, Type I error, saying that
involvement with a cult is correlated to cognitive impairment, when it is, in
fact, not related, must be avoided.�
This study used an� " level of < .05. The
program R2 by Steiger & Fouladi was employed and it indicated that with
five variables, "� < .05,
an assumed D2 =.1, a sample of 132 should yield a power of
.86018, sufficient for determining correlation.
����������� This
study examined a small part of a battery of tests that is given to individuals
entering treatment at Wellspring.� Those
instruments relevant to this study were:
1)�������� A consent form to be signed by the individual indicating his or her participation in the study found in Appendix B.
2)�������� A demographic questionnaire found in Appendix C.
3)�������� A battery of tests assembled by the Ohio University Department of Psychology in cooperation with the director of Wellspring Retreat and this researcher.� It consisted of ten instruments and is included� in Appendix D. Three were used in this study:
����������������������� a.�������� Hopkins Symptom Checklist dissociation screen
����������������������� b.�������� Beck Depression Inventory
����������������������� c.�������� Group Psychological Abuse Scale
4)�������� In addition to the Ohio Battery, the Neuropsychological Impairment Scale was also administered.
����������� It
generally took participants an average of about 2.5 hours to complete all the
items on the full battery.�
����������� In
addition, all individuals were given, among other instruments, the following at
discharge, ten days to two weeks after competing the intake battery:
����������� 1)�������� Hopkins Symptom Checklist dissociation screen
����������� 2)�������� Beck Depression Inventory
����������� 3)�������� Group Psychological Abuse Scale with one additional item: �How do you think your perceptions of concern about your group have changed since beginning treatment?�� The same five factor scale was used with the headings �much less, less, about the same, more and much more.�
����������� Scores
on the Group Psychological Abuse scale and the Neuropsychological Impairment
Scale were used to explore the relationship between intensity of group dynamics
and cognitive disruption in ex-members.�
����������� The
Ohio University Institutional Review Board granted exemption for using data
collected in the past under its auspices and is included in Appendix A.
Administration of the Neuropsychological Impairment Scale was not included in
the original IRB proposal.� It is an
extension of a program of research that predated the Ohio University
study.� Wellspring adhered to Codes of Ethics and Standards of Practice, American
Counseling Association (1995) guidelines G. 1. through G. 3. And Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code
of Conduct, American Psychological Association, 6.06 through 6.19 with
regard to the administration of this instrument.�
����������� Permission
to use the Group Psychological Abuse Scale was granted by the authors and a
sample appears in Appendix D.� Copies of
the Neuropsychological Impairment Scale were purchased from the publisher,
Western Psychological Resources.� Due to
copyright restrictions, it does not appear in the Appendices.�
Operational Definition of the Variables
����������� The
principle self report measures that were the focus of this study included the
Group Psychological Abuse scale, noted above as the operative definition of
exposure to a thought reform system and a measure of its intensity.� For this study, it is used to measure the
degree of exposure to cultic environment or �Thought Reform Intensity.�� The Neurological Impairment Scale, also
noted above is this study�s operational definition of cognitive impairment.� It is used in this study to measure the degree
of cognitive impairment the ex-member experiences. Depression as a variable for
this study is defined as elevated scores on the Beck Depression Inventory.� Dissociation is defined by elevations in
scores of a 14-item addendum to the Hopkins Symptom Checklist.�
Group Psychological Abuse Scale (GPA; Chambers. Langone, Dole & Grice, 1994 Appendix D). The GPA is a 28-item scale that seeks to examine four factors associated with abusive group environments: compliance, exploitation, mind control, and anxious dependency.� It was developed as an attempt to differentiate between groups that use practices or combinations of belief and practice that seem to be associated with deceptive persuasion techniques and those groups that do not. In the GPA, participants are asked to respond to each item in a sense of how characteristic it would be of their group.� The items are scored on a 5-point scale ranging from Not at All Characteristic (1) to Very Characteristic (5).� Total scores range from 28 to 140.� The GPA�s four subscales: Compliance, Exploitation, Mind Control, and Anxious Dependency are totaled to arrive at a Summary scale.� This scale was utilized in this study.
��������������� The GPA is a narrowly focused instrument, designed solely for members and ex-members of groups suspected of using thought reform techniques and there is little information available on its reliability and validity.� Internal consistency ranged from .70 on the Mind Control scale to .81 on the Compliance and Summary scales.� To determine the criterion validity of the GPA, The authors compared the scores of former members of groups suspected of being cultic to the arithmetic midpoint of each of the subscales and the Summary scale.� Former members of cultic groups scored higher than the midpoint on all scales and significantly higher than the midpoint on the Summary scale.� Langone. (1999) found that former members of the Boston Church of Christ, a group alleged by its ex-members to have cult-like tendencies, scored higher on the GPA Summary scale than former Catholics or former members of the InterVarsity Christian Fellowship, a much smaller group considered to be an intense but innocuous campus ministry.� The instrument seems to have concurrent criterion validity, that is, it is capable of fulling its purpose for this study, to provide a means of differentiation between the experiences of ex-members of cults from those of ex-members of other groups.
��������������� Neuropsychological Impairment Scale (NIS; O�Donnell, et al., 1994a). The NIS is a self-report measure of neuropsychological symptoms.� Its authors represent it as a screening instrument to aid in the identification, treatment and identifying progress in rehabilitation after brain injury, disease or stroke .
����������� The
NIS contains 95 items.� Five measure
test taking attitudes, ten measure affective disturbance and 80 describe
symptoms.� It provides three summary
measures, the Global Measure of Impairment (GMI), the Total Items Circled
(TIC), and the Symptom Intensity Measure (SIM).� Four Validity Checks are provided: Defensiveness (DEF), Affective
Disturbance (AFF), Response Inconsistency (INC), and a Subjective Distortion
Index (SDI). The SDI is only calculated when the client�s WAIS-R Digit Span and
Digit Symbol scaled scores are available.�
It is a comparison between the level of impairment that is expected
based on these measures of performance and the level of symptoms reported on
the NIS and will not be used for this study .
����������� Seven
areas of impairment are measured on subscales: Critical Items (CRIT), Cognitive
Efficiency (COG), Attention (ATT), Memory (MEM), Frustration Tolerance (FRO),
Learning-Verbal (L-V), and Academic Skills (ACD).�
����������� Split-Half
reliability of the first 40 and the last 40 neuropsychological items when
corrected for attenuation was .93.�
Internal consistency Cronbach�s alpha coefficients for a non-clinical
norming sample were .79 and .86 for the neuropsychiatric patient sample.
Test-retest reliability, using four groups of 25 college students was found to
range from .64 on the DEF scale to .98 for GMI scores (O�Donnell, et al.,
1994a).� It is the GMI scale that is the
primary interest in this study.
����������� Construct
validity was determined by scale intercorrelations and factor analysis.� The highest correlations were between COG
and GMI.� For this reason, this study
defines cognitive impairment as GMI scale scores. The percentage of variance in
GMI scores explained by factor analysis for patients was 41.6% and 33.4% for
the non-clinical sample, indicating the GMI scale can discriminate between
clinical and non-clinical populations.�
The largest share of the variance was found to be related to items from
the COG, ATT, MEM, L-V and ACD scales (O�Donnell, et al., 1994a).
����������� Criterion
validity for the NIS was explored using the Halstead-Reitan, the Trail Making
Test B, the Performance IQ and the Trail Making Test A, the Wechsler Memory
Scale�s General Memory Index and the WAIS-R Full Scale IQ.� Once again, the GMI was the most highly
correlated with the performance tests (O�Donnell, et al., 1994a).�
����������� These
qualities of the NIS, and particularly of the GMI subscale led to its selection
to test the hypothesis that exposure to a thought reform system is related to
impairment in cognitive ability significant enough to appear as a separate factor
in addition to depression, anxiety, and dissociation measured on the Ohio
battery.� The NIS also attempts to
determine if there is an emotional contribution (AFF) to low or high scores.�
����������� The
NIS has been used in the literature in similar ways to this study.� Losasso, (1999) used the NIS to evaluate the
effects in women of exposure to solvents in their workplace as nail
technicians.� Like this study, Losasso�s
experimental design did not include pre-exposure administration of the
instrument.� She did not test the women
before commencing work.� She compared
scores of workers with a control group and found significant differences in
cognitive efficiency, memory and learning deficits.
����������� Avants,
et al. (1997)
administered the NIS to 120 cocaine-dependent, methadone-maintained patients,
about half of whom were HIV positive.�
Those who were HIV seropositive reported significantly more impairment
than HIV-seronegative patients even when controlling for demographic and substance
use variables.
����������� Errico,
Nixon, Parsons, and Tassey (1997) found the NIS was more related to emotional
distress in 73 male alcoholics undergoing treatment, than actual cognitive
impairment.� The authors of the NIS,
O�Donnell, DeSoto, Desoto, and Reynolds, (1994b) previously found that abstinent alcoholics
reported mild cognitive impairment that eventually reached the normal range as
recovery continued.� While they reported
correlation with affective factors, they felt they were measuring cognitive impairment
in recently detoxified alcoholics that faded with continued abstinence.� Braggio, Pishkin, Parsons, & Fishkin
(1991). found NIS scores of essential alcoholics were significantly higher than
those of reactive alcoholics.���
����������� Due
to the confounding nature of affect in determining cognitive impairment shown
in the literature, the following instruments will be used to control for
emotional state in addition to the NIS�s internal Affective Disturbance Scale
(AFF).
��������������� Hopkins Symptom Checklist Dissociation Screen (HSCL; Briere and Runtz, 1990). Briere and Runtz developed this 14-item checklist to augment the Hopkins (Derogatis, et al., 1974).� Respondents were to choose a number from one (not at all) to four (extremely) indicating the intensity of their experience.� They found that this instrument had internal consistency (a=.85 and .90).� The mean was 20.95 (SD=5.80). This score was used as a �norm� for this study.� Scores more than one standard deviation above the mean were considered �above average.�� The authors found that scores correlated with childhood histories of both sexual abuse (r= .14, D < .007) and physical abuse (r = .23, D < .001). They found further that scores were congruent with the dissociation scale of the standard HSCL.� In this study, the 14-item screen, not the original full checklist is referred to as HSCL.
��������������� Beck
Depression Inventory (BDI). The BDI was recently reviewed by Richter,
Werner, Heerlein, Kraus, and Sauer (1998). They found using meta‑analyses
of studies on its psychometric properties, that its shortcomings were its high
item difficulty, its lack of representative norms, calling into question the
objectivity of its interpretation, factorial validity problems, instability of
scores over short time intervals (over the course of one day), and poor ability
to discriminate against anxiety. Its advantages were its high internal
consistency, high content validity, and especially for the purposes of this
study, its validity in differentiating between depressed and non-depressed
subjects, and its sensitivity to change. The average length of time between
administrations in this study was ten days, well out of range of its reported
instability problems. In spite of its many limitations, it remains useful in
distinguishing level of depression.�
That strength made it useful for this study.
This study intended to
determine the correlation, if any, between the intensity of the thought reform
environment to which the ex-member is exposed, measured by the GPA, and the
degree of subsequent cognitive impairment experienced by the ex-member,
indicated by the NIS.� The research
hypothesis is:
Research hypothesis: There is significant correlation between intensity of thought reform environment as measured by the GPA and cognitive impairment as measured by the NIS.�
����������� Two
additional factors need to be considered for the results to be meaningful.� First, it is necessary to establish some
rationale for accepting the veracity of ex-member recollections (GPA scores)
since so much has been written accusing them of being �disgruntled� and
�disillusioned� and therefore apt to confabulate about group conditions (Wright
& Malony 1989). Second, since affect has had a confounding effect on
studies using the NIS in the literature, (Errico, et al. 1997) the contribution
of affect relative to GPA scores on variation of NIS scores should be taken
into account.
����������� To
address the first factor, it was assumed that individuals in treatment at
Wellspring, hearing about thought reform processes and influence techniques and
after living in an environment where support was available for nearly any story
about the individual�s former group, would influence ex-members to judge their
former group more severely than they had at intake.� In order to test the stability of the GPA in quantifying
ex-members� recollection of conditions in their groups, the modified GPA
described above was administered at discharge.
Two weeks of being in an environment completely supportive of their essentially negative view of the group and learning about negative processes that they are encouraged to apply to their group should substantially increase ex-member�s scores on the GPA.�
����������� To
investigate this hypothesis, paired sample t-tests were performed using pre-
and post-treatment GPA scores.� If this
research hypothesis was supported, it would be less likely that ex-member�s
initial scores would actually represent what they experienced in the group. On
the other hand, if the null hypothesis was supported, i.e. there is no
difference between before and after treatment scores, the ex-members� scores
are more likely to reflect actual experiences in the group.
����������� To
address the second factor, the approximate emotional state of the participants
was determined at intake.� Depression
and dissociation, symptoms often observed in ex-cultists (Goldberg &
Goldberg, 1982; West & Martin, 1994; West & Singer, 1980), might also
influence NIS scores.��
The emotional state of the ex-member will have an effect on NIS scores.� The level of depression (BDI scores) and/or dissociation (HSCL scores) will be the primary contributors to a measure of cognitive impairment (NIS scores.)
����������� To
investigate these hypotheses, multiple regression will be performed to
determine the relative contribution of each variable, GPA, BDI, and HSCL
scores, to the variance in NIS scores.�
If this second research hypothesis is not supported, meaning that GPA
scores are a major contributor to variance in NIS scores, any correlations
discovered in the first part of the study will have more meaning.
����������� This
study used the BDI, HSCL, GPA and NIS scores at intake and GPA scores at both
intake and discharge.� After each
individual completed all the instruments and the demographic survey, the
batteries were scored by this investigator on a Corel Quattro Pro 7 spreadsheet
designed by this investigator.� Scores
were loaded into the statistical software program Statistical Package for
Social Sciences (SPSS; 9.0, Norusis, 1998) to take advantage of its
superior graphic and analysis procedures.
����������� Means
for the GPA and the NIS will be determined and compared to the cut-off scores
of 84 and 60, respectively.� Pearson
correlations will be calculated to explore the relationship between the two
variables.�
HA1: Both GPA and
NIS scores will be elevated.� Further,
Pearson correlations will indicate, with significance at the < .05 level, a
zero-order correlation between GPA and NIS scales.
HO1: GPA and NIS scores will not
be higher in ex-members than in the norming�
sample used by the test�s authors.�
There will not be a correlation between NIS and GPA scores.���
����������� Paired
sample t-tests will be used to examine the effect, if any, on GPA scores before
and after treatment.
����������� HA2: GPA scores will change, two-tailed, with
significance at <.05 indicating that treatment has an effect on GPA
scores.�
����������� HO2:: Treatment at Wellspring will
have no effect on GPA scores.
����������� Using
a hierarchical approach, the variables BDI, HSCL, as a group, will be regressed
on to NIS scores.� The GPA will then be
entered into the regression, to examine the relative contribution of each
variable to the variance in NIS scores.
Ha3:� GPA
scores will best explain or account for the most unique variance in NIS scores.
����������� Ho3 :
BDI, HSCL will have a confounding effect on NIS scores.
����������� In
chapter III, the process of data gathering and the instruments used in this
study was discussed. The sample consisted of 132 clients of Wellspring Retreat
and Resource Center.� All had been
screened before being admitted to treatment to insure their experiences and
symptoms were appropriate for the Center�s program emphasis.� Of all respondents, 123 completed all the
instruments in first part of this study.
����������� Data
for this study was drawn from their scores on the Neurological Impairment
Scale, the Group Psychological Abuse scale, with the Beck Depression Inventory
and the Hopkins Symptom Checklist dissociation screen used to control for
emotional state.� Scores on the GPA and
NIS were evaluated for any correlation.�
����������� The
GPA was given before and after treatment.�
Independent t-tests were used to examine the influence of treatment on
participants� recollections of conditions in the group.
����������� Multiple
regression was used to examine contributing factors (BDI, HSCL, GPA) to
variation in NIS scores.�
����������� As
presented in chapter III, the sample for this study included 132 ex-members of
groups or relationships identified through a screening interview to be
ex-members of cults, that is, groups that were very likely to have used the
thought reform techniques defined in the first chapter.� Participants participated voluntarily in the
study. They were given the option to allow the scores on their intake and
discharge tests to be included in the study or to be used only in their treatment.
They could also elect not to take the instruments at all.
����������� Demographic
material was obtained for all participants on age, gender, ethnicity,
educational level, annual income of family of origin, religious upbringing, and
nature of group.� Raw scores were
recorded from each item and means, standard deviations, and frequency counts
were computed. All analyses were performed using Norusis� Statistical
Package for Social Sciences (SPSS, Release 9.0.0, 1998). The sample used
for the analysis of means and correlations, as well as affective contribution
to elevated NIS scores, included only those participants who completed all
items (n = 123).� The sample used for
analysis of pre- and post-treatment GPA scores used the same criteria (n =
117).� These values are indicated in
Tables 1 and 1a below.� Note that the
NIS subscale designation GMI is used in tables and graphs.
����������� Missing cases (132 - 9 = 123) were due to participant
failure to respond to items.� No
participant failed to respond to all items.�
����������� The mean age of participants in the this smaller sample
than that presented in chapter III, was 33.00 years (SD = 11.31) ranging
between 16 and 65. Males numbered 39 and females, 84. Ethnicity of participants
was as follows: 113 were Caucasian (91.9%), 5�
(4.1%) were African American, 4, (3.2%) reported either Asian American
or �other� and 1 (.8%) did not answer the item.� Mean years of formal education was 14.68 (SD = 2.65).
����������� The income levels varied from 0, reported by 29
participants to $150,000, reported by 2 participants. The item was left blank
by nine participants.� No consistent
reason for this was available.� The mean
annual income for those 85 participants reporting an income greater than zero
was $44,290.� This analysis uncovered a
problem with the wording of the item.�
The questionnaire was prepared with the expectation of a younger
demographic that would have reported their parent�s income.� For older participants, the item was not
clear whether the response should indicate the participants� income or that of
their family of origin. Figure 1 indicates that annual incomes were skewed
toward the lower end.� The graph
includes those 29 participants who reported an income of zero and it indicates
that about 40 respondents reported incomes below $20,000.� Religious backgrounds of participants
included 27 who endorsed Protestant�
fundamental (22%), 32 Protestant liberal (26%), 26 Roman Catholic (21%),
4 Jewish (3%), 2 Eastern (2%), 25 other (20%), 2 reared in group (2%)� and 5 none (4%). Figure 2 provides a visual
representation of these data.
Figure 1. Income of Participants
Figure 2. Religious Background
����������� The demographic survey item
�Group Type� permitted the following responses:� (a) those groups that used the Bible in some way as a basis for
belief or practice (83), (b) those that embraced a predominately Eastern worldview
(23), (c) those participants who had been in abusive relationships or families
that fit the admission criteria (13), �other,� i.e. adolescent substance
centers, or extremist political or business related groups (4) and (d) One
participant did not answer the item.
Means and Correlations
����������� The mean of ex-member scores on the NIS was 61.2
(SD=11.63).� According to the manual for
the NIS (O�Donnell, et al, 1994a), this score indicates a level of cognitive
impairment above the normal range. Scores of 70 or more indicate significant
impairment, and 75 participants (61%) scored over 70.� Figure 3 is a histogram of NIS GMI subscale scores that indicates
the distribution was skewed by� the very
large number of scores in the vicinity of 70.�
Internal consistency was extremely high ("=.99).
����������� Mean score on the GPA was 102.4 (SD = 18.49).� The authors of the instrument (Chambers, et
al, 1994) propose that scores above 84 are likely to be indicative of a thought
reform system. This study found that 11 participants scored below 84 (10%) and
112 participants (90%) scored 85 or over.�
The line on Figure 4, a histogram of GPA scores, indicates a normal
distribution. Internal consistency was moderately high ("=.78)
����������� Ns of both measures were 127.
Figure 3. Histogram of scores on the
NIS-GMI subscale
Figure 4. Histogram of Group Psychological Abuse scores
����������� A Pearson correlation of
.24 (p=.007) was found between NIS and GPA scores (Table 6).� The null hypothesis is, therefore, rejected
in favor of the research hypothesis: there was a positive correlation between
intensity of thought reform environment as measured by the GPA and cognitive
impairment as measured by the NIS. Bartlett�s test of sphericity was
significant (p=.001)
Pre- and Post Treatment GPA Scores
����������� In an attempt to establish the veracity of the
recollections of ex-members vis-�-vis conditions in their respective groups,
paired-sample t-tests were used to measure reports of conditions in their
groups before and after treatment. Mean of GPA scores at intake was 103.26 (SD
= 18.32) and at the conclusion of treatment it was 106.43 (SD = 16.45), (n =
106).� A correlation, .798, was found
between intake and discharge GPA (p<.000) scores.�
����������� Paired samples t-test results, t = -2.92, p =
.004, d = -0.18, supported the research hypothesis that treatment did
have an effect on ex-member recollections of conditions in their groups.� The paired samples t-test was prepared using
only those participants who answered the measure both at intake and discharge
(n = 106).� This difference appeared to
be due to oversight by staff. No participants refused to answer discharge materials
when presented to them.
����������� Mean and standard deviation of the NIS�s internal AFF
scale, the BDI and the HSCL were 62.85, 29.33, and 19.58 (SDs = 12.60, 9.77,
and 11.56 respectively).Zero-order correlations between the predictors and the
NIS are reported in Table 2 to provide a basis for evaluating the results of
the regression process reported in Table 3 (Model 1 R2 = .574. Model
2 R2 = .660).
�����������
����������������������������������� HSCL������������� BDI����������������� GPA��������������� NIS
HSCL������������������������� 1.0000�����
������ .6620����� �������� .2620����� �������� .7576
������������ ���������������������� P=
.������ ���������� P=
.000��� ������� P=
.003��� ������� P=
.000
BDI����������������������������� .6620�������������� 1.0000������������ .2959�������������� .7209
����������������������������������� P= .000����������� P= . ���������������� P=
.001����������� P= .000
GPA��������������������������� .2620�������������� .2959�������������� 1.0000������������ .2431
����������������������������������� P= .003����������� P= .001����������� P=
.����������������� P= .007
NIS����������������������������� .7576���� ��������� .7209����� �������� .2431���� ��������� 1.0000
����������������������������������� P= .000��� ������� P= .000��� ������� P=
.007��� ������� P=
.
________________________________________________________________________
(Coefficient / (D.F.) /
2-tailed Significance)
Coefficients
of Measures of Affect and a Measure of Thought Reform
|
Unstandardized Coefficients |
�������������������� Standardized�������������������� Coefficients |
|
|||||
�Model������������������������������������������
B�������������� Std. Error�������� Beta����������� t����� ����Sig.�
|
|
|||||||
1 |
(Constant) |
35.861 |
2.014 |
� |
17.810 |
.000 |
||
� |
HSCL |
.602 |
.086 |
.499 |
7.024 |
.000 |
||
� |
BDI |
.392 |
.071 |
.391 |
5.496 |
.000 |
||
2 |
(Constant) |
36.064 |
3.778 |
� |
9.545 |
.000 |
||
� |
HSCL |
.602 |
.086 |
.500 |
6.971 |
.000 |
||
� |
BDI |
.393 |
.073 |
.391 |
5.405 |
.000 |
||
� |
GPA |
-2.269E-03 |
.036 |
-.004 |
-.064 |
.949 |
||
a� Dependent Variable: NIS
�����������
��������������� GPA did not contribute to variation in NIS when controlling
for HSCL and BDI. Observed power was a concern in the discussion of sample size
in chapter III.� The sample used above
was somewhat smaller, 122, due to missing cases. However, this sample size
enabled this study to find a significant multiple correlation or R.�
����������� The research hypothesis for this study was that Group
Psychological Abuse (GPA) scores would be the primary predictor of
Neuropsychological Impairment Scores (NIS) among possible confounding variables
such as measures of affect.� Instead
this analysis indicated that GPA was not related to NIS among measures of
affect.�
����������� Several participants in this investigation (13) had been
in relationships that fit the admission criteria for treatment.� Table 4 presents the means of this group and
those of the remainder of the sample (110).�
No differences between this group and the other participants was
detected when comparing HSCL, BDI, and NIS.�
GPA scores were lower in this group, and that difference was found to be
statistically significant (p = .03).�
Four participants in this group scored below 84 on the GPA.
��������������� During the course of this investigation, it appeared
that other important associations might exist between NIS and GPA.� To this end, a clustered boxplot (See Figure
5 in Appendix F) was prepared using the GPA cutoff score of 84, differentiating
between those participants who had been exposed to a thought reform environment
and those who were not so exposed, as the discriminant variable .
Table 3�
Group Type����������������� � ��������� HSCL������������� BDI����������������� GPA��������������� NIS�����
Abusive Relationships� ������������ 29.75�������������� 18.75�������������� 92.25�������������� 60.92��
Groups������������������������ � ��������� 29.33�������������� 19.58�������������� 102.7�������������� 61.20
t���������������������������������������������� .131���������������� -.174��� ��������� -2.172��������������� -.179
Sig. (two-tailed)����������������������� .896���������������� .863���������������� .032���������������
.859
����������� Standardized, or z, scores, were used to compare
variances since scales for the variables differ.� The two groups were very different in size (n = 12 vs. n = 111)
since the population was screened to increase the likelihood that those treated
at Wellspring would
be from thought reform
systems.
����������� NIS scores differed by nearly one standard deviation
between those 12 participants who did not score above 84 on the GPA, i.e. did
not indicate they had experienced a thought reform system, and those who scored
85 and above.
����������� Means and correlation between participant�s level of
reported cognitive impairment and level of exposure to thought reform were
presented.� A measure of thought reform
correlated with a measure of cognitive impairment.� Paired sample t-test results were presented indicating treatment
may have been a factor in changes in participant report of their individual
cult experience.
����������� Statistically significant zero-order correlations were
indicated between all variables in the study.�
The relative contribution of depression and dissociation, as well as
thought reform exposure, to variation in cognitive impairment scores was
analyzed using linear regression. Measures of affect were shown to have a more
unique contribution to variation in a measure of cognitive impairment than a
measure of exposure to thought reform. The results of further power analysis
necessitated by the decrease in sample size found while analyzing missing cases
were presented. Sufficient power for some analyses was determined, but power
may not have been sufficient for the multiple regression performed in this
study, due to the relatively small amount of variation in GPA scores.
����������� Further investigation prompted by the proposed research
was reported, including preliminary discriminant analysis using cut scores
developed by the authors of the instruments, to investigate whether the
differences between group means might be remarkable. A boxplot of z-scores
indicated that significant differences existed between a group of participants
exposed to levels of thought reform above the cutpoint and a group exposed to
levels below the cutpoint.
Discussion of Implications, Conclusions and Recommendations
����������� This chapter summarizes the study, and the findings presented
in the last chapter, that investigated a possible relationship between thought
reform and cognitive impairment.� Issues
raised in chapter II Review of the Literature are discussed in light of these
findings.� Topics of discussion in this
chapter are the reason for the study, implications of the demographics of the
participants in this study, a review of the hypothesis, methodology, the
results of the data analysis, the limitations of this study, possible
implications of this investigation for counselors and other human services
professionals, and suggestions for future research.
����������� The purpose of this study was to investigate the
relationship between the level of thought reform in a cult and the level of
cognitive impairment in ex-members presenting for treatment at Wellspring
Retreat and Resource Center.� The idea
for this study was suggested by an article by Singer & Ofshe (1990) in
which they observed �subtle cognitive deficiencies� in ex-members of cults.
Cognitive deficiencies, particularly difficulty making decisions and
simplistic, black and white thinking were reported by other observers (Goldberg
& Goldberg, 1982).�
����������� One hundred and thirty two clients of Wellspring Retreat
and Resource Center were administered a battery of tests that included a
demographic questionnaire, the Hopkins Symptom Checklist dissociation screen,
the Beck Depression Inventory, the Group Psychological Abuse Scale and the
Neuropsychological Impairment Scale. Of these participants, 123 completed both
the GPA and the NIS. This became the sample for demographic data and all other
investigations with the exception of the test-retest GPA which will be
discussed later.
����������� Gender data was unremarkable, females outnumbered males
85 to 39. The sample was overwhelmingly Caucasian, accounting for 113 out of
132 participants (91.9%).� This could
indicate that at least for the last several years, cult participation has been
a largely white phenomenon.� As
discussed in chapter II, the population at Wellspring had to have access to the
internet, watch news-magazine style�
programs or be willing to research cult issues on their own in
libraries, to know of the existence of the facility.�
����������� Cultural differences may also have been a factor in reducing
the non-Caucasian population. Members of some ethnic groups may be less likely
than others to admit or recognize they have been in a cult. Groups that target
non-white populations may function differently than cults.� The cultural milieu of ex-members of
predominately non-white cults might provide more support from family, extended
family or friends, for the ex-member, making treatment less of a necessity for
re-entry into society.� It may also
provide less support, making it less likely a non-white ex-member would know
treatment was available.� It is also
possible that members of other ethnic groups have more internal resources than
Caucasians, making treatment less important for their recovery.
����������� Mean years of formal education was 14.68 (SD =
2.65).� This population was fairly well
educated, yet the income level most often recorded (n = 29) was zero. Nearly 40
more, about 70 out of 133 reported an annual income at or below $20,000
suggesting over half of the participants were under-employed.�� This finding appears to confirm the
observations of some critics of cults (Martin 1993; Singer & Ofshe, 1990;
West & Martin, 1994). One possibility is that the symptoms reported in the
last chapter and discussed later in this chapter may have prevented participants
from earning the level of income that might be assumed, given their
education.� They may also have chosen to
pursue less materialistic goals for life.�
The mean annual income for those reporting an income was $44,290 (n =
85) but it was not clear whether this figure indicated the participants income
or their family of origin, since the battery was prepared on the assumption of
a younger demographic than recorded, (33.00).�
About one-half of the participants in the study were treated without
charge and might have been reluctant to divulge their income on the survey
since they had already applied for victim assistance funds on the basis of
need.� If they underreported their
income in order to get free treatment, they would underreport on the survey.
����������� Further research in this area should be conducted with a
view to exploring any hidden vocation-related costs to society due to cult
involvement. Suggestions for further study might also include determining how
long after leaving a cult it takes�
ex-members to regain the expected earning potential appropriate to their
level of education.� Although this study
was conducted using ex-members of cults rather than current members, this
finding might call Levine�s (1981) conclusions, that a cult experience was
generally helpful, into question.� At
least some ex-members do not carry those alleged benefits into their post cult
earning potential.
����������� Levine and Salter (1976) found that only 20% of current
members surveyed stayed in groups for spiritual, transcendental, or mystical
reasons.� Perhaps the fear of an
inability to adjust to life outside the group, possibly confirmed by this
study�s finding of reduced earning potential after the cult was a factor.
����������� Religious backgrounds of participants appear to indicate
that they were an exceptionally religious sample of North Americans.� Further research using general demographic
studies could confirm this assumption.�
Participant�s level of practice prior to joining the group was beyond
the scope of this study, but also bears further research.� Religious background information might have
been provided when the participant�s family had only membership in some
congregation but never actually practiced. On the other hand, this finding
might suggest that individuals with religious backgrounds might be more at risk
for cult involvement, or that a religious background before cult involvement
might be a contributor to pre-existing symptoms.� The presence of a religious background might exacerbate whatever
negative effects the group may have had on the participant in the study, making
it more likely that they would seek treatment.��
����������� Bible based groups supplied by far the largest number of
participants, 84 (68.3%) with all other types of groups and abusive
relationships accounting for only 39 (31.7%). Two participants (1.6%) did not
answer the item.� This finding was
consistent with Martin et al. (1992).�
Wellspring has historically attracted larger numbers of ex-members of
Bible based groups.�
����������� The demographics of participants suggest that considerable
difficulties would be encountered in generalizing the findings of this study to
other populations associated with cults i.e., current members and ex-members
who, for whatever reason, never sought treatment.� This study may be helpful in advancing knowledge of the needs and
challenges of some ex-members of cults.
The Hypotheses of the Study: Discussion and Comment
��������������� The research hypothesis was: The intensity of� thought reform environments is correlated to the level of cognitive impairment in ex-members. To investigate this correlation, it was necessary to determine that both scores were elevated since if they were not, any correlation would be meaningless.� The mean of ex-member scores on the NIS was 61.2 (SD = 11.69), indicating that participants were experiencing cognitive impairment outside the range of normal cognitive function. Scores of 70 or more indicate significant impairment� (O�Donnell, et al, 1994a).� Seventy five participants (60.98%) scored over 70.� This would appear to be a very serious finding.� Any clinical sample found to contain an unknown impaired sub-group of over 60 percent of the whole sample would be extraordinary. This finding supports and, to some extent quantifies, Singer (1990) who observed what they perceived to be cognitive inefficiencies in ex-members of cults they interviewed.
����������� Mean score on the GPA was 102.7 (SD = 18.42), indicating
that participants were �very likely� to have been exposed to thought reform
(Chambers, et al, 1994). Once again, this is not only elevated but is well
above the cutoff of 84.� In fact, 112
participants (90.24%) scored above the cutoff.�
Only 11 scored below 84 (9.76%).�
It must be taken into account, however, that the population from which
the respondents were drawn was pre-screened for appropriateness for
treatment.� Screening included
determining if the individual had been involved in some group or relationship
that they felt contributed to their negative symptoms.� Elevated scores were expected and should not
be considered remarkable.� The eleven
who scored below the cutpoint may have experienced some level of psychological
harm from involvement but it may not been from the experience of thought
reform, but some other avenue of emotional injury.� This situation was apparently not detected in the screening
process.
����������� The sample used in this study included 13 ex-members of
abusive relationships or families.� The
GPA scores of these individuals were significantly lower than the rest of the
sample.� This group of only 13
participants contributed a disproportional 4 of the 11 scores below the
cutpoint of 84.� It is possible that
this segment of the sample might have had other influence on the outcome of
this research.
����������� The mean reported in this study agrees with Langone� (1999), who found that GPA mean scores of
ex-members of two International Church of Christ groups were significantly
different from those of former Catholics and InterVarsity graduates, groups
generally considered intense but not necessarily as cults. In this study,
scores on both instruments indicate that respondents were experiencing
cognitive impairment and that they were very likely to have experienced a
thought reform environment.�
��������������� The results of this section of the study would seem to answer Aronoff & Lynn (1996) who were concerned that conceptual definitions of the term �cult� carry a negative connotation introducing bias into the investigative process.� Their call for an operational definition that would distribute groups along a continuum from completely non-cultic to extremely cultic would seem to be answered in the GPA scores of this study and Langone�s (1999).� The groups represented in this study clustered above the theoretical cutpoint of 84 since the population was self-selected from ex-members who had been involved in groups they felt had harmed them emotionally. The GPA may not have provided the desired continuum in this study due to the nature of the sample studied.��
����������� A correlation was found between cognitive impairment
scores (NIS) and exposure to thought reform (GPA).� The null hypothesis is, therefore, rejected in favor of the
research hypothesis: There was a positive correlation between intensity of
thought reform environment and cognitive impairment. However, the correlation
was only .24, not unusual for studies in the social sciences, statistically
significant but so weak that, at face value, should not be considered very
important.�
Veracity of Ex-member Recollections
����������� Paired-sample t-tests were used to evaluate participant
reports of conditions in their groups before and after treatment. The sample
for this investigation differed slightly from the one used in the other
analyses.� One hundred six out of 133
participants completed both the pre- and post-treatment GPA.� Participants failed to complete the discharge
tests for several reasons, usually due to apparently random factors such as
scheduling and administrative oversight and was not due to participant refusal
and self selection. Only� matched cases
were used for this calculation to increase the likelihood that the difference,
if any were found, would be due to treatment effect. Mean score on the GPA at
intake was 103.26 (SD = 18.32) and at discharge, 106.43 (SD = 16.45), (n =
106). This was expected from ex-members who would be more inclined to report
conditions in their groups more negatively after two weeks of treatment in
which considerable social support was given for a negative view of cults in
general and emotional support was available for negative feelings about their
group in particular. What is remarkable is that the difference was not
considerably higher.� Instead, pre- and
post- treatment scores only increased by 3 points.� The increase was statistically significant (p< .05) but practically, the increase may not represent an
important increase in ex-member �fabrication� of conditions inside their
groups. Treatment may have had an effect on ex-member report of conditions in
the group, but that effect would be small.
����������� This finding suggests that ex-member recollections might
be subject to negative bias, if their scores could be affected by treatment,
supporting� Anthony & Robbins, 1992;
Barker, 1995; Lewis & Bromley, 1987; Robbins & An�thony, 1980; Saliba,
1999; Shupe, et al., 1983 that ex-members respond to outside pressure to make
their experiences seem worse than they were to satisfy other�s
expectations.� It might confirm Bromley,
Shupe, & Ventimiglia, (1983), Coleman, (1984) Galanter, (1983) Lewis &
Bromley, (1987), Solomon, (1983) who alleged that organizations that support
ex-members of cults and their families apply subtle pressure to get them to
report higher levels of pathology.�
����������� Even though scores increased, the increase would not seem
to be sufficient to confirm the allegations in the literature. There may be other
explanations for the slight increase in scores after treatment.� Participant bias might have been in favor of
the group prior to treatment.� The
information received in treatment may have served simply to clarify
participant�s impressions of how the group functioned, resulting in a slightly
less biased reflection of the group.
Relative Contribution of Measures of Affect
����������� The authors of the NIS state in the manual that affect
was a factor in patient over-reporting of neuropsychological symptoms, particularly
depression (O�Donnell, et al, 1994a). To control for what they called
�affective disturbance�, they included a scale (AFF) with 10 items that address
depression and anxiety symptoms.��
����������� Participants� mean score on the AFF, was within the range indicated by the authors (> 70) that suggests affect was not a contributor to elevated NIS scores (O�Donnell, et al, 1994a).� On the other hand, participants in this study reported an above average though not clinical� levels of depression on the BDI (Richter, et al. 1998). This finding confirms West & Singer, (1980) and Wright and Malony (1989) who consistently observed depressive symptoms in ex-members.�
����������� Goldberg & Goldberg, (1982), West & Martin
(1994), West & Singer, (1980) and Singer & Ofshe, (1990) found that
dissociation or �floating,� an experience of profound ambivalence toward the
group even long after leaving, as well as occasional feelings of being back in
the group was very common in ex-members.�
Their observations were confirmed by the elevated mean score of 29.33
(SD = 29.33) found in this study using the HSCL (dissociation screen).� The authors of the HSCL (Briere & Runtz,
1990) considered this level to be above normal for both males and females.�
����������� There could be several reasons for the discrepancy in
these findings.� First, there may not be
a discrepancy.� The AFF was only eight
points below the cutpoint of 70.��
Measurement error, on the AFF, could account for the apparent
discrepancy. Both the BDI at 28 items and the HSCL at 14 items could easily be
more robust measures of their respective constructs than the AFF with only 10
items for both depression and anxiety. The HSCL dissociation screen may not be
measuring the same symptom set as the AFF.�
The BDI and HSCL were found to be very reliable in this study.��
����������� In any case, participants were clearly experiencing
significant levels of depression and dissociation.� This finding agrees with Conway & Siegelman, (1982), Lewis
& Bromley, (1987) and Martin, et al. (1992).� Conway and Siegelman found altered states, nightmares, inability
to break mental rhythms of chanting, amnesia, suicidal/self harm tendencies,
hallucinations and violent acting out increased with time of involvement in the
group.� This finding would also seem to
agree with Swartling and Swartling (1992) who found extremely high percentages
of those surveyed reported anxiety and guilt, difficulty handling emotions,
emptiness, nightmares or other disturbances of sleep, difficulty concentrating,
suicidal thoughts, and loss of a sense of identity.�
����������� Elevations in BDI and HSCL scores agree with Martin et
al. (1992).� He administered the Millon
Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI; Millon, 1983), Beck Depression Inventory,
Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL; Derogatis, et al., 1974), and the Staff Burnout Scale
(SBS-HP) also to inpatients at Wellspring Retreat and Resource Center. Ninety
five percent of participants had elevated scores on at least one measure and
means on all measures were elevated according to the instruments� published
norms.� Martin, et al. (1996) had
essentially identical results in a fol�low-up to the earlier Wellspring study,
using a different set of participants.�
This study had similar results using a third group of ex-members.
����������� The results of this study might contradict Wright and
Malony (1989) and Galanter (1983).�
Wright and Malony (1989) found ex-members� symptoms fade quickly with
time and, especially problematic in light of this study, Galanter (1983) found
that ex-members do not report greater psychopathology than nonmembers.� He proposed that� those who had been more or less forcibly removed from their
groups reported more negative feelings toward the group, a finding implicitly
contradicted by the meager change in test- retest GPA scores reported
above.� The relationship between method
of exit and reported symptoms was beyond the scope of this study and would be
an important avenue of further research.
����������� To investigate the relative contribution of affect to
cognitive impairment, affect scores and GPA were regressed on to NIS scores.
The HSCL, and the BDI were more unique contributors to variation in NIS scores
than GPA (p < .01 vs. p = .949).� Measures of affect accounted for the variation in reported
cognitive impairment.� In the light of
measures of affect, the relationship between thought reform exposure and
cognitive impairment disappeared.��
����������� This finding and the weak correlation reported above
appear to follow Lewis and Bromley (1987) who examined the same seven symptoms
as Conway and Siegelman and found no correlation between length of membership
and symptom intensity and frequency.�
There may be a weakness in experimental design in both Lewis and Bromley
and this study in using correlations to understand a relationship which may be
better studied using cutpoints.
����������� The elevated cognitive impairment scores in this study
might be due to the �effect of affect� on participant report of cognitive
impairment symptoms.� A deficiency in
the NIS instrument itself might be that it is overly sensitive to affect when
it� indicates impairment is present. On
the other hand, the NIS may be a more sophisticated indicator of the tendency
of individuals who are depressed and dissociated to make errors in logic (Beck,
et al, 1961; Briere & Runtz, 1990).�
In other words, cognitive impairment scores may have been elevated
precisely because participants were depressed and dissociated.�
����������� Other dynamics may explain the presence of cognitive
impairment and affective factors in ex-members of cults.� They might have brought this impairment into
their group experience.� They may have
also been depressed and dissociated before joining the group.� Those who find cults attractive may do so
because they are already cognitively impaired.
Discussion of Post Hoc Findings
����������� Variation in thought reform exposure as measured by the
GPA may not be as important to the study of cults than its simple presence or
absence.� The range of scores for the
GPA in this study was rather narrow, given the maximum of 140.� All but 4 cases fell between 65 and 140. Four
of the 12 scores below 84 were from survivors of cult-like abuse but not from
ex-members of cults per se. These factors might account for the low correlation
between the GPA and the NIS.� So many
participants had high GPA scores that there was little variation making it
impossible for it to be highly correlated with any other variable.
����������� The cut point (84) for thought reform exposure was used
in a additional investigation as a determinant for a study of two groups, one
exposed to thought reform and the other was not exposed. The two resulting
groups were very different in size (n = 12 vs. n = 111). This would be expected
since the population at Wellspring is screened to insure that clients had
experienced a thought reform systems.�
Thought reform as a discriminant variable for comparison of two groups
yielded an interesting contrast.� The
level of reported symptoms, dissociation, depression, and cognitive impairment,
increased dramatically for those who scored over the 84 cutpoint.� The boxplot presented in Appendix E
indicates that cognitive impairment and affect scores differed by nearly one
standard deviation between those 12 participants who did not report having
experienced a thought reform system and those 111 who did so report.� This area must remain a recommendation for
further study.
����������� Limitations to the application of this study are found in
the inherent limitations of self report measures, the nature and source of the
participants and the subject matter itself.�
����������� Self report measures are convenient for the researcher in
that data can be gathered without his or her physical presence. Even
the venerable Beck Depression Inventory, with its considerable literature, is
only a fair predictor of DSM diagnosis. The HSCL dissociation screen, the NIS
and the GPA were found in this study to be generally reliable.� This study restricted the use of the NIS and
the GPA to only those most general scales.
����������� The pool of possible participants is limited by the
complicating factors presented in chapter II.�
Current members of cults cannot be relied upon to be unbiased in their
report of conditions and affect.�
Ex-members may have axes to grind motivating them to over-report
negative aspects of their groups.� This
last limitation was addressed in part by retesting participants after
treatment.� The fact that participant
scores increased with treatment suggests that other self-reports may be
questioned.� Using participants from a
treatment facility has the limitation common to the quasi-experimental design
of� research, that is, research
performed �after the fact.�� Adequate
controls are impossible.� The lack of
history of symptoms before the participant joined the group makes the
contribution of the group dynamic, if any, impossible to detect.� Reasons for the lack of true, controlled
experimental research in thought reform are revisited below.�
����������� The population from which the participants in this study
were drawn introduced limitations to the results in several ways.� The homogeneity of the participants that may
have been a strength in the statistical analysis becomes a limitation for
generalizing the results to non-clinical populations of ex-members, or
ex-members of color.� The results of
this study would not be generalizable to current members of cults.� Any application in this area would seem to
be limited to a given ex-member having�
better insight into his or her personal behavior while in the cult.��
����������� Research on cults is particularly difficult for a variety
of reasons (Ayella 1990).� Very personal
matters motivate individuals to become involved in causes beyond
themselves.� Approaching current members
of cults with hard questions and psychological tests could reasonably be
considered sacrilegious and personally offensive.
����������� Direct experimental research on thought reform is fraught
with ethical dilemmas.� Some
sociologists assert that thought reform or brainwashing is a fictitious
concept.� They point out that it has
never been subjected to scientific scrutiny in controlled, laboratory
conditions (Barker 1995). Thought reform has not been researched in �controlled
laboratory conditions� since the early 1970's because modern research ethics do
not permit it.� Thought reform researchers
realized the potential for lasting harm to research participants (Zimbardo,
1981; Singer & Lalich, 1994). .
Implications for Counselors and Public Policy
����������� This study established the existence of a correlation
between thought reform and cognitive impairment.� As in so much of social science research, what was found in the
designed investigation may not have been as important as the peripheral, almost
collateral insights that were gained.�
This sizable sample of ex-members cults scored quite differently on a
measure of thought reform exposure than ex-members of non-cultic groups in the
literature.� Exposure to two weeks of
negative information about their groups and emotional support for negative
emotions against their groups had an effect on their scores sustaining that the
testimony of ex-members of cults concerning conditions in the group might be
questionable.�
����������� Ex-members in this study were depressed, dissociated and
a high percentage were experiencing significant levels of cognitive
impairment.� Their impairment could not
be statistically correlated to the level of thought reform when measures of
affect were factored into the relationship.
����������� Counselors should be aware that ex-members of cults who
present for treatment may do so because they may be depressed, dissociated and
experience some form of cognitive impairment.�
This study did not establish a connection between their symptoms and
cult membership and other factors besides their involvement in a cult should be
considered in their treatment.
����������� Public policy with regard to cults is problematic due to
the occasional overt acts of certain cults and cult-like groups. This study
failed to find a unique relationship between the level of thought reform in the
group and cognitive impairment in some ex-members that� might be a factor in those overt acts.�� The results of this study would imply that
the acts themselves should shape public policy rather than factors present in
ex-members.
Suggestions for Future Research
����������� Further research is called for by this study on several
fronts.� The discrepancy between
expected age and actual age of ex-members should be explored further.� Comparisons with other general demographic
studies might be fruitful in expanding our knowledge of the larger population
of ex-members who have left their groups but who did not seek treatment.� The length of time in the group should be
compared to the level of symptoms.�
����������� Cognitive impairment and thought reform should be
examined from a multicultural perspective.�
Why are so few non-white members of cults presenting for treatment.� Are non-whites less likely to be recruited
by cults?� Are there other cult-like
involvements that take the place of cults in other cultural settings such as
gangs or long term abusive relationships?�
����������� Unusually low incomes of participants suggest that
further research into how ex-members of cults rate in light of national
averages earning potential per year of formal education.� How long after leaving the cult do
ex-members attain national average earning potential, given their years of
education?� If the time is significant,
there may be a hidden vocation-related costs to individual ex-members and to
society at large from cult involvement.��
����������� Further investigation into the religious backgrounds of
ex-members of cults would provide important insights into the precursors if
any, there are to involvement in a cult.�
Are ex-cultists from more religious families than the general
population? Further research using general demographic studies could answer
this question.� Participant�s level of
practice prior to joining the group could be an important factor in cult
involvement. Does religious background have any relation to the type of group
joined?� Does it effect length of stay
in the cult?�
����������� The results of the GPA test-retest regimen should be
repeated at other times on clients at Wellspring as well as other populations
of ex-members to insure that the effect of treatment is not isolated to the
participants of this study.
����������� The measures used in this study should be given to
non-cult clinical and non-clinical ex-member control groups to further
investigate cutpoint groups for exposure to a thought reform environment and
affect on cognitive impairment.
����������� The study should be redesigned and redone using better
instrumentation for measuring cognitive impairment.� More research into the connection between affect and cognitive
impairment should influence design of the new study.
�����������
American Counseling Association. (1995). Codes of Ethics
and Standards of Practice. Alexandria, VA: Author.
American Psychiatric
Association. (1994). Diagnostic and
statistical, manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Wash�ington, DC: Author.
American Psychological
Association. (1992). Ethical principles
of psychologists and code of conduct. Washington, DC: Author.
Alexander. J. W. (1983).
Religious freedom at secular schools. Cultic
Studies Journal,2,318�320.
Anthony, D., & Robbins,
T. (1992). Law, social science and the �brainwashing� exception to the First
Amendment. Behavioral Sciences and the
Law, 10, 3�29.
Aronoff, J., & Lynn, S.
J. (1996). Psychopathology, dissociation,
and depression in treatment-seeking cult members, col�lege students, and
simulators. Unpublished master�s thesis, Ohio University, Athens, OH.
Aronoff, J., Lynn, S. J.,
& Malinoski, P. (2000). Are cultic environments harmful? Clinical Psychology Review, 1, 91-111.
Ash, S. M. (1985). Cult induced psychopathology, Part 1:
Clinical picture. Cultic Studies Journal,
2, 31�90.
Avants, S. K., Margolin, A.,
McMahon, T. J., Kosten, T. R. (1997). Association between self‑report of
cognitive impairment, HIV status, and cocaine use in a sample of cocaine‑dependent
methadone‑maintained patients. Addictive
Behaviors.22(5). 599‑611.
Ayella, M. (1990). �They
must be crazy�: Some of the difficulties in researching �cults.� American Behavioral Scientist, 33, 562�577.
Barker, E. (1995). The
scientific study of religion? You must be joking! Journal for the Scientific�
Study of Reli�gion, 34. 257�310.
Bird, F., & Reimer, B.
(1982). Participation rates in new religions and para-religious movements.
Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 21, 1�14.
Braggio, J. T., Pishkin, V.,
Parsons, O. A., Fishkin, S. M., et al. (1991). Differences between essential
and reactive alcoholics on tests of�
neuropsychological functioning and affect. Psychological Reports. 69(3, Pt 2). 1131‑1136.
Briere, J., & Runtz, M.,
(1990). Augmenting Hopkins SCL scales to measure dissociative symptoms: data
from two nonclinical samples. Journal of
Personality Assessment. 55 (1&2). 376 - 379.�����������
Bromley, D. C.. Shupe, A.
D., Jr., & Ventimiglia, J. C. (1983). The role of anecdotal atrocities in
the social construction of evil. In D. C. Bromley & J. T. Richardson
(Eds.), The brainwashing/deprogramming
contro�versy: Sociological, psychological, legal, and historical perspectives (pp.
139�160). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Chambers, W. V., Langone M.
D., Dole, A. A, & Grice, J. (1994). The Group Psychological Abuse Scale: A
measure of the varieties of cultic abuse.�
Cultic Studies Journal, 11(1),
88-117.
Clark, J. G. (1979). Cults. Journal of the American Medical Association,
242, 279�281.
Coleman, L. (1984). New
religions and the myth of mind control. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 322�325.
Conway, F., & Siegelman,
J. (1982). Information disease: Have cults created a new mental illness? Science Digest, 10, 86�92.
Conway, F., Siegelman. J.
H., Carmichael, C. W., & Coggins, J. (1986). Information disease: Effects
of covert induction and deprogramming. Update:
A Journal of New Religious Movements, 10, 43-57.
Comrey, A. L. (1970). Manual for the Comrey Personality Scales. San
Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Crown, S. (1983).
Contraindications and dangers of psychotherapy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 436� 441.
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R.
S., Rickels, K., Ulenhuth, E. H. & Covi, L. (1974) The Hopkins symptom
checklist (HSCL): A self report symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19, 1-15.
Deutsch, A., & Miller,
M.J. (1983). A clinical study of four Unification Church members. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 767�770.
Errico, A. L., Nixon, S. J.,
Parsons, O. A., Tassey, J. (1997). Screening for neuropsychological impairment
in alcoholics. In Marlatt, G. A, (Ed) & VandenBos, G. R., (Ed). Addictive behaviors: Readings on etiology,
prevention, and treatment. American Psychological Association, Washington,
DC. (pp. 309‑322).
Galanter, M. (1980).
Psychological induction into the large-group: Findings from a modern religious
sect. American Journal of Psychiatry,
137, 1574�1579.
Galanter, M. (1983).
Unification Church (�Moonie�) dropouts: Psychological readjustment after
leaving a charismatic religious group. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 984-989.
Galanter; M., & Buckley,
P. (1978). Evangelical religion and meditation: Psychotherapeutic effects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
166, 685�691.
Galanter, M., Buckley, P.,
Deutsch, A., Rabkin, R., & Rabkin, I. (1980). Large group influence for
decreased drug use: Findings from two contemporary religious sects. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse,
7, 291�304.
Galanter, M., Rabkin, R.,
Rabkin. I., & Deutsch, A. (1979). The �Moonies�: A psychological study of
conver�sion and membership in a contemporary religious sect. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 165�170.
Galanti,
G. (1993). Reflections on "brainwashing". In M. D. Langone (Ed.) Recovery from cults: Help for victims of
psychological and spiritual abuse (pp. pp. 85-103). New York: W. W. Norton.
Glass, L. L., Kirsch. M. A.,
& Parris, F. N. (1977). Psychiatric disturbances associated with Erhard
Seminars Training, I: A report of cases. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 245�247.
Goldberg, L.. &
Goldberg, W. (1982). Group work with former cultists. Social Work, 27, 163-170.
Hassan, S. (1988). Combating cult mind control. Rochester,
VT: Park Street Press.
Herman, J. (1992a). Complex
PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 5,
377�391.
Herman, J. (1992b). Trauma and Recovery. New York: Basic
Books.
Hochman, J. (1990). Miracle,
mystery, and authority: The triangle of cult indoctrination. Psychiatric Annals. 20(4). 179‑184,
187
Hyer, L., Woods, M. G.,
Bruno, R., & Boudewyns, P. (1989). The operating characteristics of the
Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 50, 554 - 567.
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle.
K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, NI.. Eshleman, S.. Wittehen, H., &
Ken�dler, K. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-lII-R psychiatric
disorders in the United States. Archives
of General Psychiatry. 51, 8�18.
Lambert, M. J., &
Bergin, A. E. (1994). The effectiveness of psychotherapy. In A. E. Bergin &
S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of
psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed.. pp. 143 � 189). New York: John
Wiley and Sons.
Langone, M. D. (1993).
Introduction. In M. D. Langone (Ed.) Recovery
from cults: Help for victims of psychological and spiritual abuse (pp.
1�21). New York: W. W. Norton.
Langone, M. D. Study Reveals Cultic Group's Abuses Boston
Church of Christ/International Churches of Christ Movement. Retrieved
August 2,1999, from American Family Foundation Web site: http://www.csj.org/studyindex/
studyresearch/study_reshboston.htm
Latkin, C. A. (1990).
Self-consciousness in members of a new religious movement: the Rajneeshees. Journal of Social Psychology, 130, 557�558.
Levine, S. V (1981). Cults
and mental health: Clinical conclusions. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 534-539.
Levine, S. V, & Salter,
N. E. (1976). Youth and contemporary religious movements: Psychosocial
findings. Canadian Psychiatric
Association Journal, 21, 41 1�420.
Lewis, J. R., & Bromley,
D. C. (1987). The cult withdrawal syndrome: A case of misattribution of cause? Jour�nal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 26, 508-522.
Lifton, R. J. (1997). Thought reform and the psychology of
totalism. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press.
Losasso, G. L. (1999).
Neurocognitive and neurosensory sequelae of exposure to low levels of organic
solvents and (meth)acrylates. Dissertation
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering,60 (6‑B).
3006.
Lottick, E. A. (1993).
Survey reveals physicians� experiences
with cults. Pennsylvania Medicine, 96, 26-28.
Lourie, J. B. (1996). Cumulative trauma: The nonproblem problem. Transactional Analysis Journal, 26, 276-283.
Maron, N. (1988). Family
environment as a factor in vulnerability to cult involvement. Cultic Studies Journal,.5, 23�43.
Martin, P. R. (1993).
Post-cult recovery: Assessment and rehabilitation. In M. D. Langone (Ed.), Recovery from cults: Help for victims of
psychological and spiritual abuse (pp. 203�231). New York: W. W.� Norton.
Martin, P. R., Aronoff, J.
B.. Zelikovsky, N. Z., Malinoski, P. T., & Lynn, S.J. (1996). Former cult members and college students:
Psychological distress and dissociation. Manuscript in preparation.
Martin, P. R., Langone, M.
D., Dole, A. A., & Wiltrout, J. (1992). Post-cult symptoms as measured by the
MCMI before and after residential treatment. Cultic Studies Journal, 9, 219�249.
Millon, T. (1983). The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory
Manual (3rd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: National Com�puter Systems.
Nicholi, A. M. (1974). A new
dimension of youth culture. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 396-401.
Norusis, M. J. (1998). SPSS
9.0 Guide to data analysis. Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
O�Donnell, W. E., DeSoto, C.
B., Desoto, J. L., & Reynolds, D. M. (1994a). The Neurological Impairment Scale manual.� Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
O�Donnell, W. E., DeSoto, C.
B., Desoto, J. L., & Reynolds, D. M. (1994b). Neuropsychological symptoms
in a cross‑sectional sample of abstinent alcoholics.�� Psychological
Reports, 75, 1475‑1484.
Richter, P., Werner, J.,
Heerlein, A., Kraus, A. & Sauer, H. (1998). On the validity of the Beck
Depression Inventory: A review. Psychopathology,� 31, 160‑168.
Robbins, T., & Anthony, D.
(1972). Getting straight with Meher Baba: A study of drug rehabilitation,
mysticism, and post-adolescent role conflict. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 11, 122�140.
Robbins, T., & Anthony,
D. (1980). The limits of �coercive persuasion� as an explanation for conversion
to authoritarian sects. Political
Psychology, 2, 22�37.
Ross, M. W. (1983). Clinical
profiles of Hare Krishna devotees. American
Journal of Psychiatry,140.
Saliba, J. A., (1999). The
earth is a dangerous place; the world view of the Aetherius Society. �Marburg Journal of Religion,
4.
Shupe, A. D., Bromley, D.
G., & Oliver, D. L., (1983). The
Anti-Cult Movement in America. New York: Garland.
Singer, M. T. & Lalich,
J. (1994). Cults in our midst. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.�
Singer, M. T. & Ofshe,
R. (1990). Thought Reform Programs and the Production of
Psychiatric Casualties. Psychiatric
Annals. 20(4). 188-193.
Singer, M. T., Temerlin, M.
K. Langone, M. D. (1990). Psychotherapy cults. Cultic Studies Journal, 7, 101‑125.
Spero, M. H. (1984). Some
pre‑ and post‑treatment characteristics of cult devotees. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 58,� 749‑750.
State of Ohio vs. Daniel D.
Kraft, Jr. Case No. 90CR 012, in the Court of Common Pleas. Lake County, Ohio.
Stein, J. (2000). The armpit
of Nevada. Time, 156, 26.
Stevens, J. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics for the
social sciences (3rd ed.). Nahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Sundberg, N. D., Latkin, C. A., Littman, R. A. Hagan, R. A. (1993). Pitfalls and pratfalls in research on an experimental community: Lessons in integrating theory and practice from the Rajneeshpuram Research Project. Journal of Community Psychology,21, 35-48.
van der Kolk, B. A. (1996). The body keeps score: Approaches to the psychobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. In van der Kolk, B. A. (Ed) & Mc Farlane, A. C. (Ed). Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society, New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Weiss, A. S. & Comrey, A. L.� (1987). Personality Characteristics of Hare Krishnas.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 51, 399-413.
West, L. J. (1990).
Persuasive techniques in contemporary cults: A public health approach. Cultic Studies Journal,� 7, 126-149
West, L. J. (1993). A
psychiatric overview of cult‑related phenomena. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 21, 1�19.
West, L. J. & Martin, P.
R. (1994). Pseudo-identity and the treatment of personality
change in victims of captivity and cults. In S. J. Lynn, J. W. Rhue. (Eds.). Dissociation: Clinical and Theoretical
Perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.
Wright, S. A., & Malony,
H. N.� (1989). Leaving
cults: the dynamics of defection. Contemporary
Psychology, 34,� 383-384 .
Zimbardo, P. G. (1981). The
ethics of inducing paranoia in an experimental setting: A reply to M. Lewis,
M.D. In IRB: A Review of Human Subjects Research, 3, 10-11.
Zimbardo, P. G. &
Hartley, C. F. (1985). Cults go to high school: A theoretical and empirical
analysis of the initial stage in the recruitment process.� Cultic Studies Journal, 2, 91-147.
Consent form Used by Participants in this Study
The �Ohio Battery� From Which This Study Was Taken
Figure
5. Boxplot of z-scores of affective and cognitive impairment variables
using GPA as discriminant variable